博碩士論文 942207002 詳細資訊




以作者查詢圖書館館藏 以作者查詢臺灣博碩士 以作者查詢全國書目 勘誤回報 、線上人數:27 、訪客IP:52.14.0.24
姓名 涂莉苹(Li-ping Tu)  查詢紙本館藏   畢業系所 認知與神經科學研究所
論文名稱 非語音訊息對中英雙語使用者的語言短期記憶的貢獻
(Non-phonoloical Contribution to Chinee-English Bilingual Verbal Short-term Memory)
相關論文
★ The influence of experience on the SNARC effect -the mapping between sequential information and spatial representation★ 中文名詞動詞的具體效果的神經相關活動
★ 字形和語意對短期記憶的影響: 從行為及腦造影實驗而來的證據★ 中文字音與字形相似度對語言短期記憶之影響
★ 中文短期記憶之字形表徵研究★ The temporal dynamics of the code-switching between alphabetic and logographic languages in unbalancedChinese-English bilinguals
★ The acquisition of Chinese literacy as a second language correlates with statistical learning of implicit transitional probability★ 以行為及腦造影證據探討英文字音及字形相似效果對中英雙語使用者之短期記憶的影響
★  The behavioral and neural correlates of orthographic lexicon and orthographic buffer in Chinese writing★ How the Brain Deals with the Asymmetry between Chinese Subject and Object Relative Clauses: Support for a Dynamic Processing Theory
★ EXPLORATION OF THE NEURAL CORRELATES OF SYNTACTIC PROCESSING IN CHINESE USING FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS★ 視知覺相關能力以及統計學習能力對中文識字學習的影響:來自以中文為母語或外語學習者的證據
★ Correlation of statistical learning of different implicit transitional probabilities with Chinese literacy acquisition in non-native adult learners★ 字形相似性、字義相似性以及視覺相似性對於中文工作記憶的影響
★ The Characteristics and Neurophysiological Correlates of Componential Visual Statistical Learning★ 金錢酬賞之來源與其分配對公平性感知的影響
檔案 [Endnote RIS 格式]    [Bibtex 格式]    [相關文章]   [文章引用]   [完整記錄]   [館藏目錄]   [檢視]  [下載]
  1. 本電子論文使用權限為同意立即開放。
  2. 已達開放權限電子全文僅授權使用者為學術研究之目的,進行個人非營利性質之檢索、閱讀、列印。
  3. 請遵守中華民國著作權法之相關規定,切勿任意重製、散佈、改作、轉貼、播送,以免觸法。

摘要(中) 過去以單一語言刺激材料為主的短期記憶研究,已支持語音的訊息是短期記憶中最主要的編碼方式。然而,鮮少有短期記憶研究探討非語音訊息的影響,更遑論使用雙語刺激材料來研究相關議題的研究。本研究設計四個行為實驗,利用探針序列回憶作業的特性,首先探討雙語刺激材料中的非語音的表徵,是否可以在短期記憶表現中彰顯出母語優勢效果。接著,我們使用中、英刺激材料依序交錯呈現的字串,去探討母語優勢效果的穩定性;同時,我們也進一步的去探討在雙語使用者的短期記憶中,是否存在著以語言類型為基礎的組織準則。在前兩個實驗中所使用的雙語刺激材料乃近乎同音,藉此方式以控制中、英配對刺激材料之間的語音複雜度,企圖去驗證當語音沒有辦法有效幫助受試者記憶時,能否依舊能觀察到短期記憶表現中的母語優勢效果。實驗一和實驗二的結果指出,即便在使用咬音抑制的狀況下,仍能觀察到短期記憶中顯著的母語優勢效果,故支持非語音表徵對雙語使用者的短期記憶表現有顯著的貢獻。實驗三和實驗四,藉由使用雙語刺激材料交錯呈現的字串,企圖檢驗雙語使用者的短期記憶中,母語優勢效果的穩定性。過去的研究者曾利用混和字串的特性,即,由兩種不同類型的刺激材料組合成的字串(如:中、英文刺激材料),並將之與探針序列回憶作業做結合的方式,發現短期記憶中的組織法則。我們也效法這樣的設計,希望能在實驗三和實驗四中去探討在雙語使用者的短期記憶中,是否存在著以語言類型為基礎的處理準則。與前兩個實驗不一致之處是,實驗三的結果並無顯示出中、英文刺激材料的短期記憶表現有顯著的不同。我們推測,實驗三的資料無法發現顯著的母語優勢效果,乃因雙語字串的使用增添了記憶作業的難度,導致受試者採取使用語音訊息的方式記憶刺激材料。在實驗四中,我們使用四個字為一組的中、英文同韻刺激材料,取代近乎同音的中、英刺激材料,是為了提高每組字串中刺激材料間的語音相似度,進而降低受試者依賴語音的程度。我們測試了兩組分別在七歲前(英文較流利)以及七歲之後(中文較流利)到達美國中、英雙語使用者。這兩組受試者的資料顯示出,短期記憶中有顯著的優勢語言效果存在,重複驗證了前兩個實驗中得到的母語優勢效果。更重要的是,這兩組資料都證實,當探針與目標皆屬同語言的狀況下,受試者的記憶表現會優於當探針與目標分屬不同語言而有語言轉換的狀況下。換句話說,即便當探針或目標屬於受試者的優勢語言的狀況下,並不會促使受試者於此狀況下的記憶表現顯著優於當探針及目標同屬受試者的非優勢語言的狀況下的記憶表現。此結果支持不同語言的刺激材料以獨立的方式被保留在雙語使用者的短期記憶中。總體而言,我們從探針序列回憶作業獲得的結果,支持非語音表徵確實會影響短期記憶中的母語優勢效果;另外,本研究結果也顯示,中、英雙語使用者的短期記憶中,確實存在以語言類型為基礎的組織/處理法則。
摘要(英) Memory research has indicated the prominent role of phonology for retaining verbal materials in short-term memory in monolinguals. However, there has been limited research addressing how non-phonological representations play roles in verbal short-term memory (VSTM), and even less is known in the context of bilingualism. The present study first aims to investigate whether the non-phonological representations give rise to the effect of first language (L1) advantage on a probed recall task. Additionally, we aim to explore the robustness of the L1 advantage effect by employing bilingual lists with mixed language modes. Moreover, we further investigate whether language-based chunking/organizing principles exist in VSTM for each language in Chinese-English bilinguals. To achieve these aims, four experiments were conducted. In the first two experiments, we manipulated the phonological complexity between pairs of approximately homophonic Chinese and English materials to examine whether the L1 advantage effect would be observed, even when the phonological information was not helpful. The results indicated that there was a significant non-phonological contribution to bilingual VSTM by demonstrating a significant L1 advantage effect in the two experiments even under articulatory suppression. In Experiments 3 and 4, we examined the robustness of the L1 advantage effect by employing bilingual lists. We also took advantage of the bilingual lists, which were composed of stimuli in two different classes (i.e., Chinese and English materials) that have been employed in the probe recalled task to reveal the organization of short-term memory, to investigate whether there are language-based chunking/organizing rules in bilingual VSTM. Inconsistent with the findings from Experiment 1 and 2, the results of Experiment 3 did not show significant difference between the recall of Chinese and English materials. We suspected that the non-significant L1 advantage resulted from the fact that participants mainly relied on phonology to retain the materials. That might be due to the increasing task difficulty by employing the bilingual lists. In Experiment 4, we employed tetrads of rhyming Chinese and English materials, instead of homophonic ones, to increase phonological similarity among the materials within each trial to avoid the participants heavily relying on phonology. We tested two groups of Chinese-English bilinguals who have immigrated to the US before or after age 7. The two groups of early- and late-arrival bilinguals have better proficiency in English and Chinese, respectively. The findings from both groups revealed the robust advantage of dominant language, which replicated the L1 advantage in previous experiments. More important, both groups demonstrated better memory performance when the probe and the target were within the same language class than when they switched from one language to the other. That is, having either the probe or the target in participants’ dominant language did not result in better recall accuracy than when both the probe and the target were in the non-dominant language. Such results suggested that materials in different languages were retained separately in VSTM. Overall, the current findings from the probed recall task support the contribution from non-phonological representations to the effect of L1 advantage, and reveal the chunking/organizing principles based on language classes in Chinese-English bilinguals’ VSTM.
關鍵字(中) ★ 語言轉換
★ 母語優勢效果
★ 雙語使用者
★ 語言短期記憶
★ 非語音表徵
關鍵字(英) ★ language switching
★ verbal short-term memory
★ non-phonological representation
★ bilingualism
★ L1 advantage effect
論文目次 1. INTRODUCITON - 1 -
1.1 TTThe Characteristics of VSTM - 3 -
1.1.1 The Phonological Component in VSTM - 3 -
1.1.1.1 The Phonological Similarity Effect - 4 -
1.1.1.2 The Word Length Effect - 5 -
1.1.1.3. Articulatory Suppression effect - 6 -
1.1.2 The Visual Component in VSTM - 8 -
1.1.2.1 Evidence from Neuropsychological Studies - 8 -
1.1.2.2 Evidence from Healthy Participants - 8 -
1.1.2.2.1 Evidence from Alphabetic Languages - 8 -
1.1.2.2.2 Evidence from Non-alphabetic Languages - 9 -
1.1.3 The Lexical-semantic Component in VSTM - 12 -
1.1.3.1 Evidence from Neuropsychological Studies. - 12 -
1.1.3.2 Evidence from Healthy Participants. - 14 -
1.1.3.2.1 The lexicality and frequency effects - 14 -
1.1.3.2.2 The semantic similarity and semantic relatedness effects - 18 -
1.2 The Memory Representations of Bilingualism - 21 -
1.2.1 The Representation of Bilingual LTM - 22 -
1.2.2 Bilingual VSTM Studies - 24 -
1.2.2.1 Phonological Component in Bilingual VSTM - 24 -
1.2.2.2 The Visuospatial/Orthographic Component in Bilingual VSTM - 28 -
1.2.2.2.1 The characteristic of Chinese characters - 28 -
1.2.2.2.2 The visuospatial/orthographic component in Chinese-English bilingualism - 30 -
1.2.2.3 The Lexical-semantic Component in Bilingual VSTM - 31 -
1.3 The Current Study - 32 -
1.3.1 The Purposes of the Present Study - 32 -
1.3.2 Serial Probed Recall Paradigm - 33 -
2. EXPERIMENT 1 - 37 -
2.1 Method - 37 -
2.1.1 Participants - 37 -
2.1.2 Materials and Design - 38 -
2.1.3 Procedures - 41 -
2.2 Results and Discussion - 43 -
3. EXPERIMENT 2 - 49 -
3.1 Method - 49 -
3.1.1 Participants - 50 -
3.1.2 Materials and design - 50 -
3.1.3 Procedures - 51 -
3.2 Results and discussions - 51 -
4. EXPERIMENT 3 - 63 -
4.1 Method - 64 -
4.1.1 Participants - 64 -
4.1.2 Materials and design - 64 -
4.1.3 Procedures - 65 -
4.2 Results and discussion - 65 -
5. EXPERIMENT 4 - 75 -
5.1 Method - 77 -
5.1.1 Participants - 77 -
5.1.1.2 Early arrivals. - 80 -
5.1.2 Materials and design - 82 -
5.1.3 Procedures - 85 -
5.2 Results and discussion - 87 -
5.2.1 Late arrivals. - 87 -
5.2.2 Early arrivals. - 95 -
6. GENERAL DISCUSSION - 106 -
6.1 A Summary of The Results in The Current Study - 106 -
6.2 Multiple Components in bilingual VSTM - 108 -
6.3 The Chunking/Organizing Rules in Bilingual VSTM - 114 -
7. CONCLUSIONS - 116 -
8. REFERENCES - 118 -
9. APPENDIX - 130 -
參考文獻 竺家寧,1999。聲韻學。台北:五南圖書出版公司。
國立台灣師範大學國音教材編輯委員會,2007。國音學。台北縣新店市 : 正中出版。
賴哲瑩 (民 97) 。Phonological and orthographic similarity affects verbal short-term memory for Chinese characters. 碩士論文。國立中央大學
涂莉苹、李俊仁(民96)。 中、英文於發音抑制的情境中對序列位置記憶效果的探討。行政院國家科學委員會補助大專學生參與專題研究計畫研究成果報告。
Allen, R., Hulme, C., (2006). Speech and language processing mechanisms in verbal serial recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 64–88.
Altarribe, J. (1992). The representation of translation equivalents in bilingual memory. Advances in psychology, 83, 157-174.
Avons, S.E., Wright, K.L., & Pammer, K. (1994). The word-length effect in probed and serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 207–232.
Baddeley, A. D. (1966). Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 362-365.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word Length and the Structure of Short-Term Memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 575-589.
Beddalay, A. D. (2003). Working memory and language: an overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189-208.
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 48, 107-112.
Baddeley, A.D. & Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation, (Vol. 8, pp. 47-90). New York: Academic Press.
Bialystok, E. (2005). Consequences of bilingualism for cognitive development. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. dr Groot (Eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches, (pp. 417-432). New York: Oxford University Press.
Broadbent , D.E. , & Broadbent , M.H.P. (1981). Recency effects in visual memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 1-15.
Brown, G. D. A., & Hulme, C. (1992). Cognitive psychology and second language processing : the role of short-term memory. Advances in psychology, 83, 105-121.
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. (2005). Computational models of working memory: putting long-term memory into context. Trends Cogn Sci, 9(11), 535-541.
Caza, N., & Belleville, S. (1999). Semantic contribution to immediate serial recall using an unlimited set of items: Evidence for a multilevel capacity view of short-term memory. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 334-338.
Chen, H.-C., & Leung, Y.-S. (1989). Patterns of lexical processing in a non-native language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 316-325.
Chen, X., Shu,P PH., Wu,P PN., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Stages in learning to pronounce Chinese characters. HPsychology in the SchoolsH,40, 115-124.
Chincotta, D., & Hoosain, R. (1995). Reading rate, articulatory suppression and bilingual digit span. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 7, 201-211.
Chincotta, D., & Underwood, G. (1996). Mother tongue, language of schooling, and bilingual digit span. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 193-208.
Chincotta, D., & Underwood, G. (1997a). Digit span and articulatory suppression: A cross-linguistic comparison. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 89-96.
Chincotta, D., & Underwood, G. (1997b). Speech rate estimates and bilingual digit span. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 325-348.
Craik, F. I. M. (1971). Primary memory. British Medical Bullietin, 27, 232-236.
Conrad, R., & Hull, A.J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 429-432.
Colle, H. A., & Welsh, A. (1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 15, 17-32.
Cowan, N., Baddeley, A. D., Elliot, E. M., & Norris, J. (2003). List composition and the word length effect in immediate recall: A comparison of localist and globalist assumptions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 74-79.
Da Costa Pinto, A. (1991). Reading rates and digit span in. bilinguals: The superiority of mother tongue. International Journal of Psychology, 26, 471-483.
David, C. (1997). English as a Global Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
DeFrancis, J., (1984) The Chinese Language. Fact and Fantasy University of Hawaii Press.
Ellis, N. C., & Hennelly, R. A. (1980). A bilingual wordlength effect: Implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 43-51.
Fabbro F. (1999): The neurolinguistics of bilingualism. An introduction. Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
Fallon, A. B., Groves, K., & Tehan, G., (1999). Phonological Similarity and Trace Degradation in the Serial Recall Task: When CAT helps RAT, but not MAN. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 301-307.
Fang, S. P., Horng, R. Y., & Tzeng, O. J.-L. (1986) Consistency effects in the Chinese character and pseudo-character naming tasks. In TKao, H. S. and Hoosain, R., Linguistics, psychology, and the Chinese language, 11-21.Hong Kong: Center of Asian Studies, University of Hong Kong.T
Fishman, J. A., (1971). The sociology of language. In J. Fishman (ed.), Advances in the sociology of language. Vol. 1. The Hague: Mouton.
Fromkin, V., and Rodman, R. (1998). An Introduction to Language. Fort Worth: Harcourt
Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Hall, M., & Peaker, S. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1–30.
Glanzer, M. & Meinzer, A. (1967) The effects of intralist activity on free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 928-935.
Gregg, V.H., Freedman, C.M. and Smith, D.K., 1989. Word frequency, articulatory suppression and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 363–374.
Greene, R. L. (1989). Immediate Serial Recall of Mixed-Modality Lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognitions, 15(2), 266-274.
Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1, 131-149.
Grosjean, F. (2001). The bilingual’s language modes. In Janet, L. N., One Mind, Two Languages: Bilingual Language Processing. Oxford: Blackwell,
Gupta, P., & MacWhinney, B. (1997). Vocabulary acquisition and verbal short-term memory: Computational and neural basis. Brain and Language, 59, 267–333.
Hanten, C., & Martin, C. (2000). Contribution of phonological and semantic short-term memory to sentence processing: Evidence from two cases of closed head injury in children. Journal of Memory & Language, 43, 335-361.
Hartley, T., & Houghton, G. (1996). A linguistically constrained model of short-term memory for nonwords. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 1-31.
Henry, L. A. (1991). The effect of word length effect and phonemic similarity in children's short term memory Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 43A, 35-52.
Henson, R. N. A., Hartley, T., Flude, B., Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (2003). Elective interference with verbal short-term memory for item and order information: Tests of a timing signal hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 1307–1334.
Henry, L. A., Turner, J. E., Smith, P. T., & Leather, C. (2000). Modality effects and the development of the word length effect in children. Memory, 8(1), 1-17.
Hitch, G.J., Halliday, M.S., Dodd, A., & Littler, J. E. (1989a). Development of rehearsal in short-term memory: Differences between pictorial and spoken stimuli. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 347­362.
Hitch, G.J., Woodin, M.E., & Baker, S. (1989b). Visual and phonological components of working memory in children. Memory and Cognition, 17, 175-185.
Hue, C. W. (1992). Recognition processes in characters naming. Language Processing in Chinese, ed. by H.-C. E. Chen and Ovid J.-L. Tzeng, 93-107. Advances in Psychology 90, Amstersam: North-Holland.
Hue, C. W., & Erickson, J.R. (1988). Short-term memory for Chinese characters and radicals. Memory and Cognition, 16 (3), 196-205.
Hue, C. W., Fang, D., Hsu, K. Y. (1990) Immediate serial recall of Chinese characters: a study of input modality effect.T Acta Psychologica, T75, 213-223.
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(6), 685-701.
Hulme, C., Newton, P., Cowan, N., Stuart, G., & Brown, G.D.A. (1999). Think before you speak: Pauses, memory search, and trace redintegration processes in verbal memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 447–463.
Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G. D. A., Martin, S., & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: Evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 1217–1232.
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.
Jefferies, E., Frankish, C., Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006) Lexical and semantic influences on item and order memory in immediate serial recognition: Evidence from a novel task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 949-964.
Jones, G. V. (1985). Deep dyslexia, imageability and ease of predication. Brain and Language, 24, 1–19.
Kintsch, W., & Buschke, H. (1969). Homophones and synonyms in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80(3), 403-407.
Kroll, J. F., & de Groot, A. M. B. (2005). Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149-174.
Kucera and Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational Analysis of Present-Day American English. Providence: Brown University Press. Retrieved August 5, 2005, from MRC Psycholinguistic Database Web Site: http://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/mrcdatabase/mrc2.html.
Ladefoged, P., & Halle, M. (1988). Some major features of the international phonetic alphabet. Language, 64, 577–582.
Landauer, T. K. (1962). Rate of implicit speech. Perception and Motor Skills, 15, 646.
Langdon, H. W., Wiig, E. H., & Nielsen, N. P. (2005). Dual-Dimension Naming Speed and Language-Dominance Ratings by Bilingual Hispanic Adults. Bilingual Research Journal, 29(2), 320-336.
Larsen, J. D., & Baddeley, A. (2003). Disruption of verbal STM by irrelevant speech, articulatory suppression, and manual tapping: do they have a common source? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 56(8), 1249-1268.
LeCompte, D. C. (1996). Irrelevant speech, serial rehearsal, and temporal distinctiveness: a new approach to the irrelevant speech effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(5), 1154-1165.
LeCompte, D. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1993). Similarity as an organizing principle in short-term memory. Memory, 1(1), 3-22.
TLee, C., Tsai, J., Su, E. C., Tzeng, O. J.-L., & Hung, D. L. (2005).T HConsistency, Regularity, and Frequency Effects in Naming Chinese CharactersH. Language and Linguistics, 6, 75-107.
Logie, R.H., Della Sala, S., Wynn, V., & Baddeley, A.D. (2000). Visual similarity effects in immediate verbal serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A(3), 626-646.
Majerus, S., & Van der Linden, M. (2003). Long-term memory effects on verbal short-term memory: a replication study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 303-310.
Manurung, R., Ritchie, G., Pain, H., Waller, A., Black, R., & O’Mara , D., (2008). Adding phonetic similarity data to a lexical database. Language Resources & Evaluation, DOI 10.1007/s10579-008-9069-5
Martin, R. C., & He, T. (2004). Semantic short-term memory and its role in sentence processing: A replication. Brain and Language, 89, 76–82.
Martin, R. C., Lesch, M. F., & Bartha, M. C. (1999). Independence of input and output phonology in word processing and short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 3-29.
Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. (1990). Repetition and verbal STM in transcortical sensory aphasia: A case study. Brain and Language, 39, 254–288.
Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. (1997). Language and auditory–verbal short-term memory impairments: Evidence for common underlying processes. Cognitive Neuropsychology,14, 641–682.
Martin, R. C., Shelton, J., & Yaffee, L. S. (1994). Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language, 33,, 83-111.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. . Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.
Mou, L. C., & Anderson, N. S. (1981). Graphemic and phonemic codings of Chinese characters in short-term retention. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 17(6), 255-258.
Murdock, B. B. J. (1967). Auditory and visual stores in short-term memory. Acta Psychologica, 27, 316-324.
Murray, D.J. (1968). Articulation and acoustic confusability in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 679-684.
National Language Committee, Ministry of Education (1998). 八十七年常用語詞調常報告書. Retrieved August 5, 2005, from National Language Committee Web Site: http://www.edu.tw/files/site_content/M0001/87news/index.htm
Neely, C. B., & LeCompte, D. C. (1999). The importance of semantic similarity to the irrelevant speech effect. Memory and Cognition, 27, 37-44.
Nimmo, L. M., & Roodenrys, S. (2005). The phonological similarity effect in serial recognition. Memory, 13, 773-784.
Palmer, S. E., & Ornstein, P. A. (1971) Role of rehearsal strategy in serial probed recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 60-66.
Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255–287.
Penney, C. G., & Butt, A. K. (1986). Within- and between-modality associations in probed recall: A test of the separate-streams hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 1-11.
Phillips, W.A., & Christie, D. F. M. (1977). Components of visual memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 117-133.
Poirier, M., & Saint-Aubin, J. (1995).Memory for related and unrelated words: Further evidence on the influence of semantic factors in immediate serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 384–404.
Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C., & Brown, G. (1993). The development of short-term memory span: separable effects of speech rate and long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56(3), 431-442.
Romani, C., Mcalpine, S., Martin, R. C. (2008). Concreteness effects in different tasks: Implications for models of short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 292–323.
Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. (1982) Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech: Implications for the structure of working memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 150-164.
Saint-Aubin, J., Ouellette, D., & Poirier, M. (2005). Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there an effect on all trials? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 171-177.
Saint-Aubin, J., & Poirier, M. (1999). Semantic Similarity and Immediate Serial Recall: Is There a Detrimental Effect on Order Information? HThe Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section AH, HHH52H, 367 – 394.
Shallice, T. & Warrington, E. K. (1970) Independent functioning of verbal memory stores: a neuropsychological study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 261–273.
Shallice, T., & Vallar, G. (1990). The impairment of auditory-verbal short-term storage. In G. Vallar, & T. Shallice (Eds.), Neuropsychological impairments of Short-term memory (pp. 11-53). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Atkinson, R. C. (1969). Storage and retrieval processes in long-term memory. Psychological Review, 76, 179-193.
Steiner, G. (1992). After Babel. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stigler, J. W., Lee, S., & Stevenson, H. W. (1986). Digit memory in Chinese and English: Evidence for a limited store. Cognition, 23, 1-20.
Tabares, J. (2008) Phonological coding in two languages: a verbal short-term memory study. Unpublished doctoral second year paper, University of California, Irvine.
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1988). Articulatory loop explanations of memory span and pronunciation rate correspondences: A cautionary note. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26, 293–296.
Thorn, A. S., & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children. Q J Exp Psychol A, 52(2), 303-324.
Thorn, A. S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Language differences in verbal short-term memory do not exclusively originate in the process of subvocal rehearsal. Psychon Bull Rev, 8(2), 357-364.
Thorn, A. S., Gathercole, S. E., & Frankish, C. R. (2002). Language familiarity effects in short-term memory: the role of output delay and long-term knowledge. Q J Exp Psychol A, 55(4), 1363-1383.
Tucker, G. Richard (1999). A Global Perspective on Bilingualism and Bilingual Education. ERIC Digest. ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics Washington DC. ERIC Identifier: ED435168
Tulving, E., & Colotla, V. A. (1970). Free recall of trilingual lists. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 86-98.
Turner, J. E., Henry, L. A., & Smith, P. T. (2000). The development of the use of long-term knowledge to assist short-term recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 53(2), 457-478.
Tzeng, O. J.-L., Wang, W. S.-Y., (1983). The first two R's. American Scientist, 71, 283-243.
Tzeng, O. J.-L., Hung, D. L., Wang, W. S.-Y., (1977). Speech recording in reading Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 621-630.
Walker, I., & Hulme, C. (1999). Concrete words are easier to recall than abstract words: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short-term serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 25, 1256–1271.
Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2003) Alphabetic Readers Quickly Acquire Orthographic Structure in Learning to read Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 183–208
Wang, M., Koda, K., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003) TAlphabetic and nonalphabetic L1 effects in English word identification: a comparison of Korean and Chinese English L2 learners. Cognition, 87 (2), 129-149.T
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1972). Neuropsychological evidence of visual storage in short-term memory tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24 (1), 30-40.
Watkins, O.C., & Watkins, M.J. (1977). Serial recall and the modality effect: Effects of word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Learning and Memory, 3, 712-718.
Yik, W.F. (1978). The effect of visual and acoustic similarity on short-term memory for Chinese words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 487-494.
指導教授 吳嫻(Denise H. Wu) 審核日期 2009-7-27
推文 facebook   plurk   twitter   funp   google   live   udn   HD   myshare   reddit   netvibes   friend   youpush   delicious   baidu   
網路書籤 Google bookmarks   del.icio.us   hemidemi   myshare   

若有論文相關問題,請聯絡國立中央大學圖書館推廣服務組 TEL:(03)422-7151轉57407,或E-mail聯絡  - 隱私權政策聲明