參考文獻 |
吳瑞屯,陳欣進(2000)。中文辨識與唸字作業中字音字義觸發效果的比較分析。《中華心理學刊》,42,65-86。
裘錫圭 (民77)。文字學概要。北京市:商務印書館。
Ashby, J., & Martin, A. (2008). Prosodic phonological representations early in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance,34, 224-236.
Ashby, J., Sanders L. D., & Kingston, J. (2008). Skilled readers begin processing sub-phonemic features by 80ms during visual word recognition: Evidence from ERPs. Biological Psychology, 80, 84-94.
Bentin, S. (1999). ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: Time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 235-260.
Berent, I., & Perfetti, C. A. (1995). A rose is a REEZ: The two-cycles model of phonology assembly in reading English. Psychological Review, 102, 146-184.
Chen, H.-C., & Shu, H. (2001). Lexical activation during the recognition of Chinese characters: Evidence against early phonological activation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 511-518.
Colheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies in information processing (pp. 151-216). London: Academic Press.
DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech: The diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
Ferrand, L., Segui, J., & Grainger, J. (1996). Masked priming of word and picture naming: the role of syllabic units. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 708-723.
Frost, R.. (1995). Phonological computation and missing vowels: mapping lexical involvement in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 398-408.
Frost, R.. (1998). Towards a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71-99.
Grainger, J., Kiyonaga, K., & Holcomb, P.J. (2006). The time-course of orthographic and phonological code activation. Psychological Science, 17, 1021–1026.
Grainger, J., & Ferrand, L. (1996). Masked orthographic and phonological priming in visual word recognition and naming: cross-task comparisons. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 623-647.
Gronau, N., & Frost, R. (1997). Prelexical phonologic computation in a deep orthography: Evidence from backward masking in Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, 107-112.
Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Chan W. H., Hsu C. H., Hung, D. L. & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). The dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: an event-related potentials study. Neuroreport, 18(2), 147-151.
Lesch, M. F., & Pollatsek, A. (1993). Automatic access of semantic information by phonological codes in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 285-294.
Liu, Y., Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2003). ERP evidence for the time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic information in Chinese meaning and pronunciation decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 1231-1247.
Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1994a). Visual access is initially phonological. 1: Evidence from associative priming by words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 107-128.
Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. (1994b). Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 2. Evidence from phonological priming by homophones and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 331-353.
Lukatela, G., Frost, S. J., & Turvey, M. (1998). Phonological priming by masked nonword primes in the lexical decision task. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 666-683.
Perfetti, C. A., Zhang, S., & Berent, I. (1992). Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a "universal" phonological principle. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds), Orthography, morphology, and meaning (pp. 227-248). Oxford, England: North-Holland.
Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S. (1996). The universal word identification reflex. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 33, 159-189.
Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24, 101-118.
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112, 43-59.
Rugg, M. D. (1984a). Event-related potentials in phonological matching tasks. Brain and Language, 23(2), 225-240.
Rugg, M. D. (1984b). Event-related potentials and the phonological processing of words and non-words. Neuropsychologia, 22(4), 435-443.
Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems. Cognition, 19, 1-30.
Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523-568.
Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y.(2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Developments, 74, 27-47.
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (1995). In defense of an onset–rime syllable structure for English. Language and Speech, 38, 127–142.
Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory and Cognition,15, 181-198.
Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1245-1265.
Ziegler, J. C, & Jacobs, A. M. (1995). Phonological information provides early sources of constraint in the processing of letter strings. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 567-593.
Ziegler, J. C, Benraïss, A., & Besson, M. (1999) From print to meaning: An electrophysiological investigation of the role of phonology in accessing word meaning. Psychophysiology, 36, 775-785.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.
|