參考文獻 |
Acheson, D. J., & Haggort, P. (2013). Stimulating the brain′s language network: Syntactic ambiguity resolution after TMS to the inferior frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(10), 1664-1677. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00430.
Adani, F. (2011). Rethinking the acquisition of relative clauses in Italian: Towards a grammatically based account. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 141-165. doi: 10.1017/S0305000909990250
Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E., & D’Esposito, M. (1998). The Variability of Human, BOLD Hemodynamic Responses. NeuroImage, 8(4), 360-369. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0369 Alpers, G. W. (2008). Eye-catching: right hemisphere attentional bias for emotional pictures.
Laterality, 13(2), 158-178. doi:10.1080/13576500701779247
Andersson, J. L. R., Hutton, C., Ashburner, J., Turner, R., & Friston, K. (2001). Modeling
geometric deformations in EPI time series. NeuroImage, 13(5), 903-919.
doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0746
Andrews, A. (1985). Studies in the syntax of relative and comparative clauses. New York:
Garland Publishing.
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (1999). Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions.
Hum Brain Mapp, 7(4), 254-266. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:4<254::AID-
HBM4>3.0.CO;2-G
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed
random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390-412. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
155
Bahlmann, J., Schubotz, R. I., & Friederici, A. D. (2008). Hierarchical artificial grammar processing engages Broca′s area. NeuroImage, 42(2), 525-534. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.249
Baldo, J. V., Katseff, S., & Dronkers, N. F. (2012). Brain regions underlying repetition and auditory-verbal short-term memory deficits in aphasia: Evidence from voxelbased lesion symptom mapping. Aphasiology, 26(3-4), 338-354. doi:10.1080/02687038.2011.602391
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Package lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-6. http://CRAN.R- project.org/package=lme4. Accessed 24 Sep, 2019.
Bates, D., Mächler, M., M Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Betancort, M., Manuel, C., & Patrick, S. (2009). The processing of subject and object relative clauses in Spanish: An eye-tracking study. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10). doi: 10.1080/17470210902866672
Bell, L. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (1994). Reading skill: Some adult comparisons. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 244-255. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.244
Ben-Shachar, M., Hendler, T., Kahn, I., Ben-Bashat, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2003). The neural reality of syntactic transformations: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychological science, 14(5), 433-440. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.01459
Ben-Shachar, M., Palti, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2004). Neural correlates of syntactic movement: converging evidence from two fMRI experiments. NeuroImage, 21(4), 1320-1336. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.027
156
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2009). The Role of Prominence Information in the Real-Time Comprehension of Transitive Constructions: A Cross-Linguistic Approach. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 19-58. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00099.x
Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Friederici, A. D., Cramon, D. Y. v., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). Who did what to whom? The neural basis of argument hierarchies during language comprehension. NeuroImage, 26(1), 221-233. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.032
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2013). Reconciling time, space and function: a new dorsal-ventral stream model of sentece comprehension. Brain and Language, 125(1), 60-76. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.010
Boukrina, O., & Graves, W. W. (2013). Neural networks underlying contributions from semantics in reading aloud. Front Hum Neurosci, 7, 518. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00518
Bouton, S., Chambon, V., Tyrand R., Guggisberg, A. G., Seeck, M., Karkar, S., et al. (2018). Focal versus distributed temporal cortex activity for speech sound category assignment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(6), E1299-E1308. doi:10.1073/pnas.1714279115
Brett, M., Johnsrude, I. S., & Owen, A. M. (2002). The problem of functional localization in the human brain. Nature, 3(3), 243-250. doi:10.1038/nrn756
Brovelli, A. (2012). Statistical analysis of single-trial Granger causality spectra. Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine, 1-10. doi:10.1155/2012/697610
Brown, C., & Hagoort, P. (1993). The Processing Nature of the N400: Evidence fiom Masked Priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 34-44. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.34
Brysbaert, M., & Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. Journal of Cognition, 1(1), 9. doi:10.5334/joc.10
157
Bulut, T. (2018). Exploration of the neural correlates of syntactic processing in Chinese using functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potentials. Doctoral dissertation. National Central University, Taiwan.
Bulut, T., Cheng, S.-K., Xu, K., Hung, D. L., & Wu, D. H. (2018). Is there a processing preference for object relative clauses in Chinese? Evidence from ERPs. Frontiers in Psychology, 1-18. doi:10.3389/fpsyg
Burholt Kristensen, L., Engberg-Pedersen, E., Højlund Nielsen, A., & Wallentin, M. (2013). The influence of context on word order processing – An fMRI study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 26(1), 73-88. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2012.05.001
Büchel, C., & Friston, K. J. (1997). Modulation of Connectivity in Visual Pathways by Attention: Cortical Interactions Evaluated with Structural Equation Modelling and fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 7(8), 768-778. doi:10.1093/cercor/7.8.768
Caplan, D., Chen, E., & Waters, G. (2008a). Task-dependent and task-independent neurovascular responses to syntactic processing. Cortex, 44(3), 257-275. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2006.06.005
Caplan, D., Stanczak, L., & Waters, G. (2008b). Syntactic and thematic constraint effects on blood oxygenation level dependent signal correlates of comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 643-656. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20044
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. (2013). Memory mechanisms supporting syntactic comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(2), 243-268. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0369-9
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 77-126.
158
Carreiras, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., Vergara, M., Cruz-Pavía, I. d. l., & Laka, I. (2010). Subject relative clauses are not universally easier to process: evidence from Basque. Cognition, 115(1), 79-92. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.012
Catani, M., Allin, M. P. G., Husain, M., Pugliese, L., Mesulam, M. M., Murray, R. M., et al. (2007). Symmetries in human brain language pathways correlate with verbal recall. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 104(43), 17163-17168. doi:10.1073/pnas.0702116104
Chan, A., Matthews, S., & Yip, V. (2011). ‘The acquisition of relative clauses in Cantonese and Mandarin’ in The Acquisition of Relative Clauses: Processing, typology and function, ed. Evan Kidd (John Benjamins Publishing Company), 197-226.
Chen, B., Ning, A., Bi, H., & Dunlap, S. (2008). Chinese subject-relative clauses are more difficult to process than the object-relative clauses. Acta Psychol (Amst), 129(1), 61-65. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.04.005
Cheng, L. L. S., & Sybesma, R. (2005). A Chinese relative. In Broekhuis, H., Corver, N., Huybregts, R., Kleinhenz, U., & Koster, J. (Eds.), Organizaing grammar: Linguistics studies in honor of Henk van Riemsdijk. 69-76. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, Germany.
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris:
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). Creating language: Integrating evolution, acquisition,
and processing. The MIT press.
Christensen, K. R., Kizach, J., & Nyvad, A. M. (2013). The processing of syntactic islands - An
fMRI study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 26(2), 239-251.
doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2012.08.002
Chwilla, D. J., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (1995). The N400 as a function of the level of
processing. Psychophysiology, 32(3), 274-285. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb02956.x 159
Clahsen, H., & Featherston, S. (1999). Antecedent Priming at Trace Positions: Evidence from German Scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 415-437. doi: 10.1007/s10936-006-9041-8
Cohen, L., & Lacques, M. (1996). Click monitoring revisited: An on-line study of sentence comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 24(1), 94-102. doi:10.3758/BF03197275
Cointepas, Y., Mangin, J.-F., Garnero, L., Poline, J.-B., & Benali, H. (2001). BrainVISA: software platform for visualization and analysis of multi-modality brain data. NeuroImage, 13(6), 98. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(01)91441-7
Constable, R. T., Pugh, K. R., Berroya, E., Mencl, W. E., Westerveld, M., Ni, W., et al. (2004). Sentence complexity and input modality effects in sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 22(1), 11-21. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.001
Cooke, A., Zurif, E. B., DeVita, C., Alsop, D., Koenig, P., Detre, J., et al. (2002). Neural basis for sentence comprehension: grammatical and short-term memory components. Hum Brain Mapp, 15(2), 80-94. doi:10.1002/hbm.10006
Cope, T. E., Sohoglu, E., Sedley, W., Patterson, K., Jone, P. S., Wiggins, J. et al. (2017). Evidence for causal top-down frontal contributions to predictive processes in speech perception. Nature Communications, 8(1), 2154. doi:10.1038/s41467-017-01958-7
Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken- language comprehension: immediate effects of verb-based thematic constraints. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 30(2), 498-513. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.498
Davatzikos, C., Prince, J. L., & Bryan, R. N. (1996). Image registration based on boundary mapping. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 15(1), 112-115. doi:10.1109/42.481446
160
De Luca, M., Beckmann, C. F., De Stefano, N., Matthews, P. M., & Smith, S. M. (2006). fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain. NeuroImage, 29(4), 1359-1367. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.035
Denes, G. (2015). Neural plasticity across the lifespan: How the brain can change. Routledge. 95-121.
Deshpande, G., LaConte, S., James, G. A., & Hu, S. P. (2009). Multivariate granger causality analysis of fMRI data. Hum Brain Mapp, 30(4), 1361-1373. doi:10.1002/hbm.20606
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2005). A new look at the acquisition of relative clauses. Language, 81(4). 882-906. doi:10.1353/lan.2005.0169.
Ding, M., Bressler, S. L., Yang, W., & Liang, H. (2000). Short-window spectral analysis of cortical event-related potentials by adaptive multivariate autoregressive modeling: data preprocessing, model validation, and variability assessment. Biological Cybernetics, 83(1), 35-45. doi:10.1007/s004229900137
Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D., Jr., Redfern, B. B., & Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition, 92(1- 2), 145-177. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002
Drury, H. A., Essen, D. C. V., Anderson, C. H., Lee, C. W., Coogan, T. A., & Lewis, J. W. (1996). Computerized Mappings of the Cerebral Cortex: A Multiresolution Flattening Method and a Surface-Based Coordinate System. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(1), 1-28. doi:10.1162/jocn.1996.8.1.1
Dryer, M. S. (1992). The Greenbergian Word Order Correlations. Language, 68(1), 81-138. Dryer, M. S. (2005). Relationship between the order of object and verb and the relative clause
and noun. In Haspelmath, M., Dryer, M. S., Gil, D., & Comrie, B. (Eds), The World Atlas of Language Structures, 394-395. Oxford University Process, New York.
161
Dryer, M. S. (2013). Order of Relative Clause and Noun. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath. (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
Du, P. F. (2013). Internal and External Factors in the Processing of Chinese Relative Clauses. Master dissertation. National Chung Cheng University, Taiwan.
Duann, J. R., Jung, Z. P., Kuo, W. R., Yeh, T. C., Makeig, S., Hsieh, J. C et al. (2002). Single-trial variablity in event-related BOLD signals. NeuroImage, 15(4), 823-835. 2002. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1049
Duann, J.-R., Ide, J. S., Luo, X., & Li, C.-s. R. (2009). Functional connectivity delineates distinct roles of the inferior frontal cortex and presupplementary motor area in stop signal inhibition. J Neurosci, 29(32), 10171-10179. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1300-09.2009
Dubois, J., Benders, M., Lazeyras, F., Borradori-Tolsa, C., Leuchter, R. H., Mangin, J. F., et al. (2010). Structural asymmetries of perisylvian regions in the preterm newborn. NeuroImage, 52(1), 32-42. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.054
Dubois, J., . . . Wallois, F. (2013). Syllabic discrimination in premature human infants prior to complete formation of cortical layers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 110(12), 4846-4851. doi:10.1073/pnas.1212220110
Edward E, S., & Jonides, J. (1998). Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 95, 12061-12068.
Enfield, N. J. (2004). ′Adjectives in Lao′, In Adjective Classes: A Cross-Linguistic Typology, ed. R. M. W. Dixon, & A. Aikhenvald. Oxford University Press, 323-347.
162
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 429-492. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999094X
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). Right words and left words: electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 8(3). 373-392. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00036-1
Federmeier, K., D., Wlotko, E. W., De Ochoa-Dewald, E., & Kutas, M. (2007). Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing. Brain Research, 1146, 75-84. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.101
Fengler, A., Meyer, L., & Friederici, A. D. (2016). How the brain attunes to sentence processing: Relating behavior, structure, and function. NeuroImage, 129, 268-278. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.012
Ferreira, F., & Clifton, C. (1986). The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 25(3), 348-368. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(86)90006-9
Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (1990). Use of verb information in syntactic parsing: Evidence from eye movements and word-by-word self-paced reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16(4), 555-568. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.16.4.555
Fiebach, C. J., Rissman, J., D′ Esposito, M. (2006). Modulation of inferotemporal cortex activation during verbal working memory maintenance. Neuron. 51(2), 251-261. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.007
Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Syntactic working memory and the establishment of filler-gap dependencies: insights from ERPs and fMRI. Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 321-338. doi:10.1023/A:1010447102554
163
Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B. H., & Dale, A. M. (1999). High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp, 8(4), 272- 284. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-4
Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303(5656), 377-380. doi:10.1126/science.1089401
Fodor, J. D. (1978). Parsing strategies and constraints on transformations. Linguistic Inquiry, 9, 427-473.
Fodor, J. D. (1989). Empty categories in sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3,155-209.
Fodor, J. D., Bever, T. G., & Garrett, M. F. (1974). The psychology of language: an introduction to psycholinguistics and generative grammar. McGraw-Hill.
Fodor, J. D., & Inoue, A. (1994). The diagnosis and cure of garden paths. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23, 407-435. doi:10.1007/BF02143947
Ford, M. (1983). A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity thoughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22(2), 203-218. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90156-1
Frazier, L. (1987). Syntactic processing: Evidence from dutch. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 5(4), 519-559.
Friederici, A. D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiol Rev, 91(4), 1357-1392. doi:10.1152/physrev.00006.2011
Friederici, A. D. (2012). The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends Cogn Sci, 16(5), 262-268. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.001
Friederici, A. D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R. I., & Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: functional localization and structural
164
connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 103(7), 2458-2463.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0509389103
Friederici, A. D., & Kotz, S. A. (2003). The brain basis of syntactic processes: functional
imaging and lesion studies. NeuroImage, 20(S8-S17). doi:10.1016/S1053-
8119(03)00522-6
Friederici, A. D., Makuuchi, M., & Bahlmann, J. (2009). The role of the posterior superior
temporal cortex in sentence comprehension. Neuroreport, 20(6), 563-568.
doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283297dee
Friederici, A. D., Meyer, M., & Cramon, D. Y. v. (2000). Auditory language comprehension: an
event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain
Lang, 74(2), 289-300. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2313
Friederici, A. D., Rüschemeyer, S.-A., Hahne, A., & Fiebach, C. J. (2003). The role of left
inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral Cortex, 13(2), 170-177. doi:10.1093/cercor/13.2.170
Friston, K. J., Ashburner, J., Frith, C. D., Poline, J.-B., Heather, J. D., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1995a). Spatial registrationand normalizationof images. Hum Brain Mapp, 2, 165-189. doi:10.1002/hbm.460030303
Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Worsley, K. J., Poline, J.-P., Frith, C. D., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1995b). Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp, 2(4), 189-210. doi:10.1002/hbm.460020402
165
Friston, K. J., Ungerleider, L. G., & P. Jezzard, R. T. (1994). Characterizing modulatory interactions between V1 and V2 in human cortex with fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 2(4), 211-224. doi:10.1002/hbm.460020403
Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(5), 263-279. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11001841
Gerard, L. D., & Scarborough, D. L. (1989). Language-specific lexical access of homographs by bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(2), 305-315. doi: 10.1037/0278- 7393.15.2.305
Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 1-76. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00034-1
Gibson, E., & Wu, H. H. I. (2013). Processing Chinese relative clauses in context. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(1-2), 1-31. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.536656
Girard, P., & Bullier, J. (1989). Visual activity in area V2 during reversible inactivation of area 17 in the macaque monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 62(6), 1287-1302. doi:10.1152/jn.1989.62.6.1287
Goebela, R., Roebroecka, A., Kimb, D.-S., & Formisano, E. (2003). Investigating directed cortical interactions in time-resolved fMRI data using vector autoregressive modeling and Granger causality mapping. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 21(10), 1251-1261. doi:10.1016/j.mri.2003.08.026
Gordon, P. C., & Lowder, M. W. (2012). Complex Sentence Processing: A Review of Theoretical Perspectives on the Comprehension of Relative Clauses. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(7), 403-415. doi:10.1002/lnc3.347
166
Granger, C. W. J. (1969). Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econometrica, 37(3), 424-438. doi:10.2307/1912791
Griffanti, L., Douaud, G., Bijsterbosch, J., Evangelisti, S., Alfaro-Almagro, F., Glasser, M. F., et al. (2017). Hand classification of fMRI ICA noise components. NeuroImage, 154, 188- 205. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.036
Grodner, D., & Gibson, E. (2005). Consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input for sentenial complexity. Cognitive Science, 29(2), 261-290. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_7
Grodzinsky, Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Neuroimaging of syntax and syntactic processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 16(2), 240-246. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.007
Hagoort, P. (2003). Interplay between Syntax and Semantics during Sentence Comprehension: ERP Effects of Combining Syntactic and Semantic Violations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(6), 883-899. doi:10.1162/089892903322370807
Hagoort, P. (2014). Nodes and networks in the neural architecture for language: Broca′s region and beyond. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 136-141. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.013
Hagoort, P. (2017). The core and beyond the language-ready brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 81(Pt B), 194-204. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.048
Hagoort, P. (2019). The neurobiology of language beyond single-word processing. Science, 366, 55-58.
Hagoort, P., Indefrey, P., Brown, C., Herzog, H., Steinmetz, H., & Seitz, R. d. J. (1999). The neural circuitry involved in the reading of German words and pseudowords: a PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(4), 383-398. doi:10.1162/089892999563490
167
Handwerker, D. A., Ollinger, J. M., & D′Esposito, M. (2004). Variation of BOLD hemodynamic responses across subjects and brain regions and their effects on statistical analyses. NeuroImage, 21(4), 1639-1651. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.029
Hartwigsen, G., Henseler, I., Stockert, A., Wawrzyniak, M., Wendt, C., Klingbeil, J. et al. (2017). Integration demands modulate effective connectivity in a fronto-temporal network for contextual sentence integration. NeuroImage, 147. 812-824. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.026
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of Language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569-1579. doi:10.1126/science.298.5598.1569
Hawkins, J. A. (1999). Processing Complexity and Fill-Gap Dependencies across Grammars. Language, 75, 244-285.
Hawkins, J. A. (2004). Efficiency and Complexity in Grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
He, W., & Chen, B.-G. (2013). The role of animacy in Chinese relative clause processing. Acta Psychol (Amst), 144(1), 145-153. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.022
Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F. (1990). Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: Implications for attention and eye movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16(3), 417-429. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.16.3.417
Hengeveld, K. (1992). ′Parts of speech′, In Layered Structure and Reference in a Functional Perspective, ed. M. Fortescue, P. Harder & L. Kristofferson (John Benjamins Publishing Company), 29-56.
168
Hsu, N. C-C, Hermon, G., & Zukowski, A. (2009). Young children’s production of head-final relative clauses: Elicited production data from Chinese children. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 18(4), 23-60. doi:10.1007/s10831-009-9047-y.
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci, 8, 393-402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113
Holmes, V. M., & O′Regan, J. K. (1981). Eye Fixation Patterns during the Reading of Relative- Clause Sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 417-430.
Hsiao, F., & Gibson, E. (2003). Processing relative clauses in Chinese. Cognition, 90(1), 3-27. doi:10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00124-0
Hsiao, Y., & MacDonald, M. C. (2013). Experience and generalization in a connectionist model of Mandarin Chinese relative clause processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00767
Hsieh, Y., Boland, J. E., Zhang, Y., & Yan, M. (2009). Limited syntactic parallelism in Chinese ambiguity resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(7-8), 1227-1264. doi: 10.1080/01690960802050375
Hsu, C. C., Hurewitz, F., & Phillips, C. (2006). Contextual and syntactic cues for processing head-final relative clauses in Chinese. Paper presented at the 19th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing. New York, NY.
Hutton, C., Bork, A., Josephs, O., Deichmann, R., Ashburner, J., & Turner, R. (2002). Image distortion correction in fMRI: a quantitative evaluation. NeuroImage, 16(1), 217-240. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1054
169
Jäger, L., Chen, Z., Li, Q., Lin, C.-J. C., & Vasishth, S. (2015). The subject-relative advantage in Chinese: Evidence for expectation-based processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 79-80, 97-120. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2014.10.005
Jelinek, E. (1995). ′Quantification in Straits Salish′, In Quantification in Natural Languages, ed. E. Bach, E. Jelinek, A. Kratzer & B. Partee (Kluwer), 487-540.
Jin, L. (2016). Typological evidence of mixed word orders in mandarin and its motivation. Chinese Language Learning, 6(3), 3-11.
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychological review, 99(1), 122.
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., & Thulborn, K. R. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274(5284), 114-116. doi:10.1126/science.274.5284.114
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 133-156. doi:10.1016/s0749-596x(03)00023-8
Keenan, E. L., & Comrie, B. (1977). Noun Phrase Accessibility and Universal Grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 8(1), 63-99.
Keller, T. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2001). The neural bases of sentence comprehension: a fMRI examination of syntactic and lexical processing. Cerebral Cortex, 11(3), 223-237. doi:10.1093/cercor/11.3.223
Kemmerer, D. (2012). The cross-linguistic prevalence of SOV and SVO word orders reflects the sequential and hierarchical representation of action in Broca’s area. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(1), 50-66. doi:10.1002/lnc3.322
170
Kempen, G., & Hoenkamp, E. (1987). An Incremental Proceduml Grammar for Sentence Formulation. Cognitive Science, 11(2), 201-258. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1102_5
Kim, J.-J., Kim, M. S., Lee, J. S., Lee, D. S., Lee, M. C., & Kwon, J. S. (2002). Dissociation of working memory processing associated with native and second languages: PET investigation. NeuroImage, 15(4), 879-891. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1025
King, J., & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: the role of working memory. Memory and Language, 30(5), 580-602. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(91)90027-H
King, J., & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word- and clause-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(3), 376-395. doi:10.1162/jocn.1995.7.3.376
Kuhnke, P., Meyer, L., Friederici, A. D., & Hartwigsen, G. (2017). Left posterior inferior frontal gyrus is causally involved in reordering during sentence processing. NeuroImage, 148, 254-263. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.013
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01560-6
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13). doi:10.18637/jss.v082.i13
Kwon, N., Kluender, R., Kutas, M., & Polinsky, M. (2013). Subject/object processing asymmetries in Korean relative clauses: evidence from ERP. Language, 89(3), 537-585. doi:10.1353/lan.2013.0044
171
Kwon, N., Lee, Y., Gordon, P. C., Kluender, R., & Polinsky, M. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic factors affecting subject/object asymmetry: An eye-tracking study of prenominal relative clauses in Korean. Language, 86(3). 546-582. doi:10.1353/lan.2010.0006
Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 920-933. doi:10.1038/nrn2532
Leff, A. P., Schofield, T. M., Crinion, J. T., Seghier, M. L., Grogan, A., Green, D. W., et al. (2009). The left superior temporal gyrus is a shared substrate for auditory short-term memory and speech comprehension: evidence from 210 patients with stroke. Brain, 132(Pt 12), 3401-3410. doi:10.1093/brain/awp273
Leroy, F., Glasel, H., Dubois, J., Hertz-Pannier, L., Thirion, B., Mangin, J. F., et al. (2011). Early maturation of the linguistic dorsal pathway in human infants. J Neurosci, 31(4), 1500- 1506. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4141-10.2011
Li, X., Liu, P., & Rayner, K. (2011). Eye movement guidance in Chinese reading: is there a preferred viewing location? Vision Res, 51(10), 1146-1156. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.004
Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1974). ′Historical change of word order. a case study in Chinese and its implications′. in Historical linguistics, ed. Anderson, J. M., & Jone, C. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A (1981). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press. 116.
Lin, C.-J. C. (2014). Effect of thematic order on the comprehension of Chinese relative clauses. Lingua, 140, 180-206. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2013.12.003
172
Lin, C.-J. C. (2018). Subject prominence and processing dependencies in prenominal relative clauses: The comprehension of possessive relative clauses and adjunct relative clauses in Mandarin Chinese. Language, 94(4), 758-797. doi:10.1353/lan.2018.0053.
Lin, C.-J. C., & Bever, T. G. (2006). Subject Preference in the Processing of Relative Clauses in Chinese. Proceedings of the 25th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 254-260.
Lin, Y., & Garnsey, S. (2011). Animacy and the resolution of temporary ambiguity in relative clause comprehension in Mandarin. In Yamashita, H., Hirose, Y, & Packard J. L (Eds.), Processing and producing head-final structures (pp. 241-275). Springer: Nature.
Love, T., D., S., Walensky, M., & Zurif, E. B. (2008). How left inferior frontal cortex participates in syntactic processing: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 107(3), 203-219. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.11.004
MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109(1), 35-54. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.109.1.35
MacWhinney, B., & Pleh, C. (1988). The Processing of Restrictive Relative Clauses in Hungarian. Cognition, 29, 95-141.
Maddieson, I. (1984). Patterns of Sounds. Cambridge University Press.
Mahmoudzadeh, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Fournier, M., Kongolo, G., Goudjil, S., Dubois, J.,
et al. (2013). Syllabic discrimination in premature human infants prior to complete formation of cortical layers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 110(12), 4846-4851. doi:10.1073/pnas.1212220110
173
Mak, W., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2006). Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4), 466-490. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.01.001
Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 50-68. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2837
Mancini, S., Molinaro, N., Davidson, D. J., Avilés, A., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Person and the syntax–discourse interface: An eye-tracking study of agreement. Journal of Memory and Language, 76, 141-157. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2014.06.010
McKeown, M. J., Makeig, S., Brown, G. G., Jun, T. P., Kindermann, S. S., Bell, A. J., Sejnowski, T. J. (1998). Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components. Human Brain Mapping, 6(3). 160-188. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097- 0193(1998)6:3<160::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-1
Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K., & Friederici, A. D. (1995). Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Memory & Cognition, 23(4), 477-494. doi:10.3758/BF03197249
Meyer, L., Obleser, J., Anwander, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2012). Linking ordering in Broca′s area to storage in left temporo-parietal regions: the case of sentence processing. NeuroImage, 62(3), 1987-1998. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.052
Meyer, L., Grigutsch, M., Schmuck, N., Gaston, P., & Friederici, A. D. (2015). Frontal-posterior theta oscillations reflect memory retrieval during sentence comprehension. Cortex, 71, 205-218. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.027
Mielke, J. (2007). The emergence of distinctive features. Oxford University Press.
174
Miller, G. A., & Chomsky, N. (1963). Finitary models of langauge users. In D. Luce (ed.). Handbook of Mathematical Psychology. John Willey & Sons. 2-419.
Ming, T. (2010). The relative position of demonstratives and relative clauses in Mandarin Chinese. NACCL22&IACL18 Processings, 323-340.
Mitchell, D. C., Cuetos, F., Corley, M. M. B., & Brysbaert, M. (1995). Exposure-based models of human parsing: Evidence for the use of coarse-grained (nonlexical) statistical records. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24(6), 469-488. doi:10.1007/BF02143162
Miyamoto, E. T., & Nakamura, M. (2003). Subject/object asymmetries in the processing of relative clauses in Japanese. Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL), 22, 345-355.
Moro, A., Tettamanti, M., Perani, D., Donati, C., Cappa, S. F., & Fazio, F. (2001). Syntax and the brain: disentangling grammar by selective anomalies. NeuroImage, 13, 110-118. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0668
Muller, H. M., King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1997). Event-related potentials elicited by spoken relative clauses. Cognitive Brain Research, 5(3), 193-203. doi:10.1016/S0926- 6410(96)00070-5
Musso, M., Moro, A., Glauche, V., Rijntjes, M., Reichenbach, J., Büchel, C., et al. (2003). Broca’s area and the language instinct. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 774-781. doi:10.1038/nn1077
Näätänen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M., Huotilainen, M., Livonen, A. et al. (1997). Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain response. Nature, 385(6615), 432-434. doi:10.1038/385432a0
175
Nakano, Y., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2002). Antecedent priming at trace positions in Japanese long-distance scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 531-571.
Nakamura, K., Kuo, W. J., Pegado, F., Cohen, L., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Dehaene, S. (2012). Universal brain systems for recognizing word shapes and handwriting gestures during reading. Proc Natl Acad Sci, 109(50), 20762–20767. doi:10.1073/pnas.1217749109
Newman, A. J., Pancheva, R., Ozawa, K., Neville, H. J., & Ullman, M. T. (2001). An event- related fMRI study of syntactic and semantic violations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 339-364. doi:10.1023/A:1010499119393
Newman, A. J., Supalla, T., Hauser, P., Newport, E. L., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Dissociating neural subsystems for grammar by contrasting word order and inflection. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 107, 7539-7544. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003174107
Nishitani, N., Schürmann, M., Amunts, K., & Hari, R. (2005). Broca’s region: from action to language. Physiology, 20(1), 60-69. doi:10.1152/physiol.00043.2004
Novais-Santos, S., Gee, J., Shah, M., Troiani, V., Work, M., & Grossman, M. (2007). Resolving sentence ambiguity with planning and working memory resources: Evidence from fMRI. NeuroImage, 37(1), 361-378. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.077
O′Grady, W. (1997). Syntactic Development. University of Chicago Press.
Opitz, B., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Brain correlates of language learning: the neuronal
dissociation of rule-based versus similarity-based learning. J Neurosci, 24(39), 8436-
8440. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2220-04.2004
Ouden, D.-B. d., Saur, D., Mader, W., Schelter, B., Lukic, S., Wali, E., . . . Thompson, C. K.
(2012). Network odulation during complex syntactc processing. NeuroImage, 59(1), 815- 823. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.057
176
Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., & Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp, 25(1), 46-59. doi:10.1002/hbm.20131
Packard, J. L., Ye, Z., & Zhou, X. (2010). Filler-Gap Processing in Mandarin Relative Clauses: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. 38, 219-240. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-9213- 7_11
Pallier, C., Devauchelle, A. D., & Dehaene, S. (2011). Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 108(6), 2522-2527. doi:10.1073/pnas.1018711108
Palmer, M., Chiou, F.D., Xue, N., & Xia, F. (2005). The Penn Chinese Treebank Corpus. Papoutsi, M., Stamatakis, E. A., Griffiths, J., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (2011). Is
left fronto-temporal connectivity essential for syntax? Effective connectivity, tractography and performance in left-hemisphere damaged patients. NeuroImage, 58(2), 656-664. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.036
Pattamadilok, C., Dehaene, S., & Pallier, C. (2016). A role for left inferior frontal and posterior superior temporal cortex in extracting a syntactic tree from a sentence. Cortex, 75, 44-55. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.012
Perani, D., Saccuman, M. C., Scifo, P., Anwander, A., Spada, D., Baldoli, C., et al. (2011). Neural language networks at birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 108(38), 16056-16061. doi:10.1073/pnas.1102991108
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y. & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112(1), 43- 59. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.43
177
Perlbarg, V., Bellec, P., Anton, J.-L., Pélégrini-Issac, M., Doyon, J., & Benali, H. (2007). CORSICA: Correction of structured noise in fMRI by automatic identification of ICA components. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 25(1), 35-46. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910340409
Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Abada, S. H. (2005). ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 22(3), 407-428. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.012
Pollatsek, A., Rayner, K., & Henderson, J. M. (1990). Role of spatial location in integration of pictorial information across saccades. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16(1), 199- 210. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.16.1.199
Pu, M. M. (2007). The distribution of relative clauses in Chinese discourse. Discourse Processes, 43(1), 25-53. doi:10.1207/s15326950dp4301_2
Pugh, K. R., Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L., Fletcher, J. M., et al. (1996). Predicting reading performance from neuroimaging profiles: the cerebral basis of phonological effects in printed word identification. NeuroImage, e(e), S455. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(96)80457-5
Qiao, X., Shen, L., & Forster, K. (2012). Relative clause processing in Mandarin: evidence from the maze task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(4), 611-630. doi:10.1080/01690965.2011.578394
Rasekh-Mahand, M., Alizadeh-Sahraie, M., & Izadifar, R. (2016). A corpus-based analysis of relative clause extraposition in Persian. Ampersand, 3, 21-31. doi:10.1016/j.amper.2016.02.001
178
Rauschecker, J. P., & Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat Neurosci, 12, 718-724. doi:10.1038/nn.2331
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol Bull, 124(3), 372-422. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.124.3.372
Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., & Raney, G. E. (1996). Eye movement control in reading: A comparison of two types of models. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22(5),1188- 1200. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.22.5.1188
Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Morris, R. K., Schmauder, A. R., & Clifton, C. (1989). Eye movements and on-line language comprehension processes. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4(3-4), S121-S149. doi:10.1080/01690968908406362
R Development Core Team. (2011). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 3- 900051-07-0. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Reali, F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2007). Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(1), 1-23. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.014
Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105(1), 125-157. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.105.1.125
Roebroeck, A., Formisano, E., & Goebel, R. (2005). Mapping directed influence over the brain using Granger causality and fMRI. NeuroImage, 25(1), 230-242. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.017
179
Rodd, J., M., Longe, O. A., Randall, B., & Tyler L. K. (2010). The functional organisation of the fronto-temporal language system: Evidence from syntactic and semantic ambiguity. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1324-1335. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.035
Rogalsky, C., & Hickok, G. (2010). The role of Broca′s area in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1664-1680. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21530
Rogalsky, C., Matchin, W., & Hickok, G. (2008). Braoca′s area, sentence comprehension, and working memory: an fMRI study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 1-13. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.014.2008
Roland, D., Dick, F., & Elman, J. L. (2007). Frequency of basic English grammatical structures: A corpus analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(3), 348-379. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.002
Rolheiser, T., Stamatakis, E. A., & Tyler, L. K. (2011). Dynamic processing in the human language system: synergy between the arcuate fascicle and extreme capsule. J Neurosci, 31, 16949-16957. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2725-11.2011
Rueckl, J. G., Paz-Alonso, P. M., Molfese, P. J., Kuo, W.-J., Bick, A., Frost, S. J., et al. (2015). Universal brain signature of proficient reading: Evidence from four contrasting languages. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 112(50), 15510-15515. doi:10.1073/pnas.1509321112
Sakai, K. L. (2005). Language acquisition and brain development. Science, 310(5749), 815-819. doi:10.1126/science.1113530
Sandell, J. H., & Schiller, P. H. (1982). Effect of cooling area 18 on striate cortex cells in the squirrel monkey. J Neurophysiol, 48(1), 38-48. doi:10.1152/jn.1982.48.1.38
180
Santi, A., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2010). fMRI adaptation dissociates syntactic complexity dimensions. NeuroImage, 51(4), 1285-1293. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.034
Sato, J. R., Takahashi, D. Y., Arcuri, S. M., Sameshima, K., Morettin, P. A., & Baccalá, L. A. (2009). Frequency domain connectivity identification: an application of partial directed coherence in fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp, 30(2), 452-461. doi:10.1002/hbm.20513
Saur, D., Kreher, B. W., Schnell, S., mmerer, D. K., Kellmeyer, P., Vry, M.-S., et al. (2008). Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(46), 18035–18040. doi:10.1073/pnas.0805234105
Saur, D., Schelter, B., Schnell, S., Kratochvil, D., Küpper, H., Kellmeyer, P., et al. (2010). Combining functional and anatomical connectivity reveals brain networks for auditory language comprehension. NeuroImage, 49(4), 3187-3197. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.009
Schriefers, H., Angela, D. F., & Katja, K. (1995). The processing of locally ambiguous relative clauses in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(4), 499-520. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1995.1023
Sheng, Y. & Wu, F. (2012). Audience design affects classifiers positioning in Chinese relative clauses: Evidence from spoken corpus and sentence-production data. Poster presented at the 25th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing. March 14-16. New York, NY.
Skeide, M. A., Brauer, J., & Friederici, A. D. (2014). Syntax gradually segregates from semantics in the developing brain. NeuroImage, 100, 106-111. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.080
181
Skeide, M. A., Brauer, J., & Friederici, A. D. (2016). Brain Functional and Structural Predictors of Language Performance. Cereb Cortex, 26(5), 2127-2139. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv042
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1998). Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 85(20), 12061–12068. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.20.12061
Sreenivasan, K. K., Curtis, C. E., D′ Esposito, M. (2014). Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memory. Trens Cogn Sci., 18(2), 82-89. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001
Staub, A. (2010). Eye movements and processing difficulty in object relative clauses. Cognition, 116(1), 71-86. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.04.002
Stephan, K. E., Marshall, J. C., Penny, W. D., Friston, K. J., & Fink, G. R. (2007). Interhemispheric integration of visual processing during task-driven lateralization. J Neurosci, 27(13), 3512-3522. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4766-06.2007
Stoeckel, C., Gough, P. M., Watkins, K. E., & Devlin, J. T. (2009). Supramarginal gyrus involvement in visual word recognition. Cortex, 45(9). 1091-1096. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.12.004
Stowe, L. A., Broere, C. C. A. J., Paans, A. M. J., Wijers, A. A., Mulder, G., Vaalburg, W., et al. (1998). Localizing components of a complex task: sentence processing and working memory. Neuroreport, 9(13), 2995-2999. doi:10.1097/00001756-199809140-00014
Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Rauch, S. (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language, 52(3), 452-473. doi:10.1006/brln.1996.0024
Su, Y. C. (2006). Word order effect in children’s garden path of relative clauses. Concentric: Studies in Linguistics, 32: 33-57.
182
Su, Y. C., Lee, S. E., & Chung, Y. M. (2007). Asyntactic thematic role assignment by Mandarin aphasics: a test of the Trace-Deletion Hypothesis and the Double Dependency Hypothesis. Brain Lang, 101(1), 1-18. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.12.001
Sung, Y.-T., Cha, J.-H., Tu, J.-Y., Wu, M.-D., & Lin, W.-C. (2016). Investigating the Processing of Relative Clauses in Mandarin Chinese: Evidence from Eye-Movement Data. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45(5), 1089-1113. doi:10.1007/s10936-015-9394-y
Tai, James. H. Y. (1985). ′On the Change From SVO to SOV in Chinese.′ in Readings in Chinese Transformational Syntax, ed. Shou-hsin Teng, Taiwan: Crane Publishing Company, pp. 429-444.
Tai, James H. Y., & Du, Pei-Fen. (2016). ′Nage′ before vs. after Relative Clauses in Chinese: A Processing View. Commemorative Essays for Professor Yuen Ren Chao: Father of Modern Chinese Linguistics, ed. by Andy Chin Chi-on, Kwok Bit-chee, & Benjamin K. Tsou, 275-292. Taipei: Crane Publishing.
Tettamanti, M., Alkadhi, H., Moro, A., Perani, D., Kollias, S., & Weniger, D. (2002). Neural Correlates for the Acquisition of Natural Language Syntax. NeuroImage, 17(2), 700-709. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1201
Thompson, P., & Toga, A. W. (1996). A surface-based technique for warping threedimensional images of the brain. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 15(4), 402-417. doi:10.1109/42.511745
Tohka, J., Foerde, K., Aron, A. R., Tom, S. M., Toga, A. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2008). Automatic independent component labeling for artifact removal in fMRI. NeuroImage, 39(3), 1227- 1245. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.013
183
Tosun, D., & Prince, J. L. (2008). A geometry-driven optical flow warping for spatial normalization of cortical surfaces. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 27(12), 1739- 1753.
Traxler, M. J., Morris, R. K., & Seely, R. E. (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 69-90. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2836
Traxler, M. J., Williams, R. S., Blozis, S. A., & Morris, R. K. (2005). Working memory, animacy, and verb class in the processing of relative clauses. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(2), 204-224. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.010
Tsai, W. T. D. (1997). On the absence of island effects. Tshing Hua Journal of Chinese Studies, 27, 125-149.
Ueno, M., & Garnsey, S. M. (2008). An ERP study of the processing of subject and object relative clauses in Japanese. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(5), 646-688. doi:10.1080/01690960701653501
Ueno, M., & Kluender, R. (2003). Event-related brain indices of Japanese scrambling. Brain and Language, 86(2), 243-271. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00543-6
Vasishth, S., Chen, Z., Li, Q., & Guo, G. (2013). Processing Chinese relative clauses: evidence for the subject-relative advantage. PLoS One, 8(10), e77006. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077006
Waters, G.S., Caplan, D., 1996. The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 51–79. doi:10.1080/713755607
Warren, P. (2012). Introducing Psycholinguistics. Cambridge University Press.
184
Weiller, C., Musso, M., Rijntjes, M., & Saur, D. (2009). Please don′t underestimate the ventral pathway in language. Trends Cogn Sci, 13, 369-370; 370-361. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.007
Weiss, S., Mueller, H. M., Schack, B., King, J. W., Kutas, Martha, & Rappelsberger, P. (2005). Increased neuronal communication accompanying sentence comprehension. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57(2), 129-141. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.013
Well, J. B., Christiansen, M. H., Race, D. S., Acheson, D. J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Experience and sentence processing: statistical learning and relative clause comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 58(2), 250-271. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.08.002
Wu, F. (2009). Factors affecting relative clause processing in Mandarin (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA.
Wu, F. (2011). Frequency issues of classifier configurations for processing Mandarin object- extracted relative clauses: A corpus study. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 7(2), 203-227. doi: 10.1515/cllt.2011.010.
Wu, F., Kaiser, E., & Andersen, E. (2012). Animacy effects in Chinese relative clause processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(10), 1489-1524. doi:10.1080/01690965.2011.614423
Wu, Z., & Juffs, A. (2016). What kind of priming is most effective in the processing of relative clauses in context? Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America, 1, 37. doi:10.3765/plsa.v1i0.3728
Xu, K., Duann, J. R., Huang, Hung, D. L., & Wu, D. H. (2019). Preference or Object Relative Clauses in Chinese Sentence Comprehension: Evidence from Online Self-paced Reading Time. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2210. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02210.
185
Xu, K., Denise, H. W., & Duann, J. R. (2019). Enhanced left inferior frontal to left superior temporal effective connectivity for complex sentence comprehension: fMRI evidence from Chinese relative clause processing. Brain and Language. Accepted.
Yang, C. L., & Perfetti, C. A. (2006). Contextual Constraints on the Comprehension of Relative Clause Sentences in Chinese: ERPs Evidence. Language and Linguistics, 7(3), 697-730.
Yang, C.L., Perfetti, C. A. & Liu, Y. (2010). Sentence integration processes: An ERP study of Chinese sentence comprehension with relative clauses. Brain and Language, 112, 85-100. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.10.005
Zempeni M. Z., Renken, R., Hoeks, J. C. J., Hoogduin, J. M., & Stowe, L. A. (2007). Semantic ambiguity processing in sentence context: Evidence from event-related fMRI. NeuroImage, 34(3), 1270-1279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.048
Zhang, Y. X., Zhang, H. C., & Hua, S. (2000). A study on the processing of ambiguous phrases in Chinese. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 32, 13-19. doi:10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01323.
Zhang, N. (2008). Gapless relative clauses as clausal licensers of relational nouns. Language and Linguistics, 9 (). 1003-1026.
Zheng, Y., Doñamayor, N., & Münte, T. F. (2014). Brain Network of Semantic Integration in Sentence Reading: Insights from Independent Component Analysis and Graph
Theoretical Analysis. Human Brain mapping, 35(2), 367-376. doi:10.1002/hbm.22182 |