參考文獻 |
Adelman, J. S., Brown, G. D., & Quesada, J. F. (2006). Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychol Sci, 17(9), 814-823. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01787.x
Armstrong, B. C., & Plaut, D. C. (2016). Disparate semantic ambiguity effects from semantic processing dynamics rather than qualitative task differences. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(7), 940-966. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2016.1171366
Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 133(2), 283.
Barber, H. A., Otten, L. J., Kousta, S.-T., & Vigliocco, G. (2013). Concreteness in word processing: ERP and behavioral effects in a lexical decision task. Brain and language, 125(1), 47-53.
Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought (pp. 129-163). https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499968.007
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv, 1406.5823.
Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology, 60(4), 343-355.
Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M.-H., Echallier, J.-F., & Pernier, J. (1999). ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(3), 235-260.
Besson, M., Kutas, M., & Petten, C. V. (1992). An event-related potential (ERP) analysis of semantic congruity and repetition effects in sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4(2), 132-149.
Bolger, D. J., Balass, M., Landen, E., & Perfetti, C. A. (2008). Context Variation and Definitions in Learning the Meanings of Words: An Instance-Based Learning Approach. Discourse Processes, 45(2), 122-159. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638530701792826
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior research methods, 41(4), 977-990.
Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2016). How Many Words Do We Know? Practical Estimates of Vocabulary Size Dependent on Word Definition, the Degree of Language Input and the Participant′s Age. Front Psychol, 7, 1116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01116
Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PloS one, 5(6), e10729.
Cevoli, B., Watkins, C., & Rastle, K. (2021). What is semantic diversity and why does it facilitate visual word recognition? Behav Res Methods, 53(1), 247-263. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01440-1
Chan, K. Y., & Vitevitch, M. S. (2009). The influence of the phonological neighborhood clustering coefficient on spoken word recognition. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 35(6), 1934-1949. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016902
Chapman, C. A., & Martin, R. C. (2021). Effects of semantic diversity and word frequency on single word processing. J Exp Psychol Gen. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001123
Chapman, R. M., & Bragdon, H. R. (1964). Evoked responses to numerical and non-numerical visual stimuli while problem solving. nature, 203(4950), 1155-1157.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological review, 108(1), 204.
Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological review, 117(3), 713.
Davis, P. A. (1939). Effects of acoustic stimuli on the waking human brain. Journal of neurophysiology, 2(6), 494-499.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of psychiatric research, 12(3), 189-198.
Forster, K. I., & Chambers, S. M. (1973). Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(6), 627-635.
Galbraith, R. C., & Underwood, B. J. (1973). Perceived frequency of concrete and abstract words. Mem Cognit, 1(1), 56-60. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198068
Goldstein, R., & Vitevitch, M. S. (2014). The influence of clustering coefficient on word-learning: how groups of similar sounding words facilitate acquisition. Front Psychol, 5, 1307. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01307
Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. Psychological review, 103(3), 518.
Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2013). Semantic diversity: a measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of words. Behav Res Methods, 45(3), 718-730. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0278-x
Hoffman, P., Ralph, M. L., & Rogers, T. (2021). Semantic diversity is best measured with unscaled vectors: Reply to Cevoli, Watkins and Rastle (2020).
Hoffman, P., & Woollams, A. M. (2015). Opposing effects of semantic diversity in lexical and semantic relatedness decisions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 41(2), 385-402. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038995
Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2006). On the time course of visual word recognition: an event-related potential investigation using masked repetition priming. J Cogn Neurosci, 18(10), 1631-1643. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1631
Hollis, G. (2020). Delineating linguistic contexts, and the validity of context diversity as a measure of a word′s contextual variability. Journal of Memory and Language, 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104146
Howes, D. H., & Solomon, R. L. (1951). Visual duration threshold as a function of word-probability. Journal of experimental psychology, 41(6), 401.
Hsiao, Y., & Nation, K. (2018). Semantic diversity, frequency and the development of lexical quality in children’s word reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 103, 114-126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.08.005
Hsu, C.-H., Lee, C.-Y., & Liang, W.-K. (2016). An improved method for measuring mismatch negativity using ensemble empirical mode decomposition. Journal of neuroscience methods, 264, 78-85.
Hsu, C. H., Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. Y., & Tzeng, O. J. (2009). Orthographic combinability and phonological consistency effects in reading Chinese phonograms: an event-related potential study. Brain Lang, 108(1), 56-66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.002
Hua, M. S., Chang, B. S., Lin, K. N., Yang, C. M., Lu, H. J., & Chen, H. Y. (2005). Wechsler Memory Scale- III (Chinese Version): Administration and Scoring Manual. Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation.
Johns, B. T., Dye, M., & Jones, M. N. (2016). The influence of contextual diversity on word learning. Psychon Bull Rev, 23(4), 1214-1220. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0980-7
Johns, B. T., Gruenenfelder, T. M., Pisoni, D. B., & Jones, M. N. (2012). Effects of word frequency, contextual diversity, and semantic distinctiveness on spoken word recognition. J Acoust Soc Am, 132(2), EL74-80. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4731641
Johns, B. T., Sheppard, C. L., Jones, M. N., & Taler, V. (2016). The Role of Semantic Diversity in Word Recognition across Aging and Bilingualism. Front Psychol, 7, 703. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00703
Jones, M. N., Dye, M., & Johns, B. T. (2017). Context as an Organizing Principle of the Lexicon. In (pp. 239-283). https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.008
Jones, M. N., Johns, B. T., & Recchia, G. (2012). The role of semantic diversity in lexical organization. Can J Exp Psychol, 66(2), 115-124. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026727
Kunzetsova, A., Brockhoff, P., & Christensen, R. (2017). lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effect models. Journal of statistical software, 82(1), 1-26.
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu Rev Psychol, 62, 621-647. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203-205.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. nature, 307(5947), 161-163. https://doi.org/10.1038/307161a0
Laszlo, S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). The N400 as a snapshot of interactive processing: Evidence from regression analyses of orthographic neighbor and lexical associate effects. Psychophysiology, 48(2), 176-186. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01058.x
Lee, C.-L., & Federmeier, K. D. J. B. R. (2006). To mind the mind: An event-related potential study of word class and semantic ambiguity. 1081(1), 191-202.
Mak, M. H., Hsiao, Y., & Nation, K. (2021). Anchoring and contextual variation in the early stages of incidental word learning during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 118, 104203. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/kf96e
MATLAB. (2014). version 8.4.0.150421 (R2014b), Natick, Massachusetts: The MathWorks Inc.
Mestres-Missé, A., Münte, T. F., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2014). Mapping concrete and abstract meanings to new words using verbal contexts. Second Language Research, 30(2), 191-223. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658313512668
Mkrtychian, N., Blagovechtchenski, E., Kurmakaeva, D., Gnedykh, D., Kostromina, S., & Shtyrov, Y. (2019). Concrete vs. Abstract Semantics: From Mental Representations to Functional Brain Mapping. Front Hum Neurosci, 13, 267. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00267
Monsch, A. U., Bondi, M. W., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Katzman, R., & Thal, L. J. (1992). Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol, 49(12), 1253-1258. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1992.00530360051017
Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological review, 76(2), 165.
Nelson, D. L., Bennett, D. J., Gee, N. R., Schreiber, T. A., & McKinney, V. M. (1993). Implicit memory: effects of network size and interconnectivity on cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(4), 747.
Noppeney, U., & Price, C. J. (2004). Retrieval of abstract semantics. Neuroimage, 22(1), 164-170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.010
Pagán, A., & Nation, K. (2019). Learning Words Via Reading: Contextual Diversity, Spacing, and Retrieval Effects in Adults. Cogn Sci, 43(1). https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12705
Perea, M., Soares, A. P., & Comesana, M. (2013). Contextual diversity is a main determinant of word identification times in young readers. J Exp Child Psychol, 116(1), 37-44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.014
Pexman, P. M., Heard, A., Lloyd, E., & Yap, M. J. (2017). The Calgary semantic decision project: concrete/abstract decision data for 10,000 English words. Behav Res Methods, 49(2), 407-417. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0720-6
Pigada, M., & Schmitt, N. (2006). Vocabulary acquisition from extensive reading: A case study. Reading in a foreign language, 18(1), 1-28.
Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological review, 103(1), 56.
Plummer, P., Perea, M., & Rayner, K. (2014). The influence of contextual diversity on eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(1), 275.
Qiu, M., & Johns, B. T. (2020). Semantic diversity in paired-associate learning: Further evidence for the information accumulation perspective of cognitive aging. Psychon Bull Rev, 27(1), 114-121. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01691-w
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.r-project.org/
Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P., Shaoul, C., Milin, P., & Baayen, H. (2014). The myth of cognitive decline: non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning. Top Cogn Sci, 6(1), 5-42. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12078
Rayner, K., & Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & cognition, 14(3), 191-201.
Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological review, 105(1), 125.
Rodd, J., Gaskell, G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2002). Making Sense of Semantic Ambiguity: Semantic Competition in Lexical Access. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(2), 245-266. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2810
Rubenstein, H., Garfield, L., & Millikan, J. A. (1970). Homographic entries in the internal lexicon. Journal of verbal learning verbal behavior, 9(5), 487-494.
Schmitt, N., & Schmitt, D. (2020). Vocabulary in language teaching. Cambridge university press.
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Stowe, R. W. (1989). Context Availability and the Processing of Abstract and Concrete Words in Sentences. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(1), 114-126. https://doi.org/10.2307/748013
Segui, J., Mehler, J., Frauenfelder, U., & Morton, J. (1982). The word frequency effect and lexical access. Neuropsychologia, 20(6), 615-627.
Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J., & John, E. (1965). Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science, 150(3700), 1187-1188.
Tsai, P. S., Yu, B. H., Lee, C. Y., Tzeng, O. J., Hung, D. L., & Wu, D. H. (2009). An event-related potential study of the concreteness effect between Chinese nouns and verbs. Brain Res, 1253, 149-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.10.080
Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory & cognition, 18(4), 380-393.
Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J. (2012). Prediction during language comprehension: benefits, costs, and ERP components. Int J Psychophysiol, 83(2), 176-190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.015
Vergara-Martinez, M., Comesana, M., & Perea, M. (2017). The ERP signature of the contextual diversity effect in visual word recognition. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci, 17(3), 461-474. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0491-7
Verkoeijen, P. P., Rikers, R. M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2004). Detrimental influence of contextual change on spacing effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, Cognition, 30(4), 796.
Walter, W. G., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V., McCallum, W., & Winter, A. (1964). Contingent negative variation: an electric sign of sensori-motor association and expectancy in the human brain. nature, 203(4943), 380-384.
Webb, S. (2007). The Effects of Repetition on Vocabulary Knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 28(1), 46-65. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/aml048
West, W. C., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level processing of concrete and abstract words: an electrophysiological investigation. J Cogn Neurosci, 12(6), 1024-1037. https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290051137558
Wulff, D. U., De Deyne, S., Jones, M. N., Mata, R., & Aging Lexicon, C. (2019). New Perspectives on the Aging Lexicon. Trends Cogn Sci, 23(8), 686-698. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.003
Yap, M. J., Lim, G. Y., & Pexman, P. M. (2015). Semantic richness effects in lexical decision: The role of feedback. Mem Cognit, 43(8), 1148-1167. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0536-0 |