參考文獻 |
Agostino, A., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2010).
Executive functions underlying multiplicative
reasoning: Problem type matters. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 105(4), 286-305.
Ardila, A., Pineda, D., & Rosselli, M. (2000). Correlation
between intelligence test scores and executive
function measures. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 15(1), 31-36.
Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in
cognitive control. The Neuroscientist, 13(3), 214-228.
Biscaldi, M., Fischer, B., & Hartnegg, K. (2000).
Voluntary saccadic control in dyslexia. Perception, 29
(5), 509-521.
Briand, K. A., Strallow, D., Hening, W., Poizner, H., &
Sereno, A. B. (1999). Control of voluntary and
reflexive saccades in Parkinson's disease.
Experimental Brain Research, 129(1), 38-48.
Bruno, A., Brambati, S. M., Perani, D., & Morrone, M. C.
(2006). Development of saccadic suppression in
children. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(3), 1011-1017.
Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a
predictor of children's mathematics ability:
Inhibition, switching, and working memory.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.
Chiau, H.-Y., Tseng, P., Su, J.-H., Hsu, W.-Y., Tzeng, O.
J. L., Hung, D. L., et al. (submitted). Trial type
probability modulates the cost of anti-saccades.
Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive
functions. In E. Bialystok & F. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan
cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 70-95). NY:
Oxford University Press.
Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2007). Inhibition of
action, thought, and emotion: A selective
neurobiological review. Journal of the American
Association of Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12
(3), 99-114.
Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., &
Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe:
The organization of goal-directed behavior. Cognitive
Psychology, 30(3), 257-303.
Eenshuistra, R. M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Weidema, M. A., &
van der Molen, M. W. (2007). Developmental changes in
oculomotor control and working-memory efficiency. Acta
Psychologica, 124(1), 139-158.
Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M.
M., Hamby, A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution
of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills
in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology,
26(1), 465-486.
Evdokimidis, I., Smyrnis, N., Constantinidis, T. S.,
Stefanis, N. C., Avramopoulos, D., Paximadis, C., et
al. (2002). The anti-saccade task in a sample of 2,006
young men. I. Normal population characteristics.
Experimental Brain Research, 147(1), 45-52.
Everling, S., Dorris, M. C., Klein, R. M., & Munoz, D. P.
(1999). Role of primate superior colliculus in
preparation and execution of anti-saccades and pro-
saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(7), 2740-2754.
Everling, S., & Fischer, B. (1998). The antisaccade: A
review of basic research and clinical studies.
Neuropsychologia, 36(9), 885-899.
Everling, S., & Munoz, D. P. (2000). Neuronal correlates
for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and
anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field.
Journal of Neuroscience, 20(1), 387-400.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I.,
& Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional
networks. Neuroimage, 26(2), 471-479.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner,
M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence
of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 14(3), 340-347.
Fan, J., & Posner, M. (2004). Human attentional networks.
Psychiatrische Praxis, 31 Suppl 2, S210-214.
Fischer, B., Biscaldi, M., & Gezeck, S. (1997). On the
development of voluntary and reflexive components in
human saccade generation. Brain Research, 754, 285-297.
Fischer, B., Biscaldi, M., & Otto, P. (1993). Saccadic eye
movements of dyslexic adult subjects.
Neuropsychologia, 31(9), 887-906.
Frank, M. C., Vul, E., & Johnson, S. P. (2009).
Development of infants' attention to faces during the
first year. Cognition, 110(2), 160-170.
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among
inhibition and interference control functions: A
latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 133(1), 101-135.
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E.,
Defries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all
executive functions are related to intelligence.
Psychological Science, 17(2), 172-179.
Fukushima, J., Hatta, T., & Fukushima, K. (2000).
Development of voluntary control of saccadic eye
movements. I. Age-related changes in normal children.
Brain & development, 22(3), 173-180.
Geier, C. F., Terwilliger, R., Teslovich, T., Velanova,
K., & Luna, B. (2009). Immaturities in reward
processing and its influence on inhibitory control in
adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1613-1629.
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O.,
Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al.
(1999). Brain development during childhood and
adolescence: A developmental MRI study. Nature
Neuroscience, 2(10), 861-863.
Gmeindl, L., Rontal, A., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2005).
Strategic modulation of the fixation-offset effect:
Dissociable effects of target probability on pro-
saccades and anti-saccades. Experimental Brain
Research, 164(2), 194-204.
Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2002). Programming of
endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a
competitive integration model. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(5),
1039-1054.
Grossmann, T., Striano, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2007).
Developmental changes in infants' processing of happy
and angry facial expressions: A neurobehavioral study.
Brain and Cognition, 64(1), 30-41.
Hanisch, C., Radach, R., Holtkamp, K., Herpertz-Dahlmann,
B., & Konrad, K. (2006). Oculomotor inhibition in
children with and without attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Neural
Transmission, 113(5), 671-684.
Irving, E. L., Tajik-Parvinchi, D. J., Lillakas, L.,
Gonzalez, E. G., & Steinbach, M. J. (2009). Mixed pro-
and anti-saccade performance in children and adults.
Brain Research, 1255, 67-74.
Johnson, M. H. (1995). The inhibition of automatic
saccades in early infancy. Developmental
Psychobiology, 28, 281–291.
Juan, C. H., Muggleton, N. G., Tzeng, O. J., Hung, D. L.,
Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2008). Segregation of visual
selection and saccades in human frontal eye fields.
Cerebral Cortex, 18(10), 2410-2415.
Karatekin, C. (2006). Improving antisaccade performance in
adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD). Experimental Brain Research, 174(2),
324-341.
Karatekin, C., Bingham, C., & White, T. (2009). Oculomotor
and pupillometric indices of pro- and anti-saccade
performance in youth-onset psychosis and attention
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin.
Klein, C., & Foerster, F. (2001). Development of pro-
saccade and anti-saccade task performance in
participants aged 6 to 26 years. Psychophysiology, 38
(2), 179-189.
Klein, C. H., Raschke, A., & Brandenbusch, A. (2003).
Development of pro- and anti-saccades in children with
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and
healthy controls. Psychophysiology, 40(1), 17-28.
Kramer, A. F., Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. M., & Cassavaugh,
N. D. (2005). Development of attentional and
oculomotor control. Developmental Psychology, 41(5),
760-772.
Kristjansson, A., Chen, Y., & Nakayama, K. (2001). Less
attention is more in the preparation of anti-saccades,
but not pro-saccades. Nature Neuroscience, 4(10), 1037-
1042.
Kristjansson, A., Vandenbroucke, M. W., & Driver, J.
(2004). When pros become cons for anti- versus pro-
saccades: Factors with opposite or common effects on
different saccade types. Experimental Brain Research,
155(2), 231-244.
Liu, C. L., Chiau, H. Y., Tseng, P., Hung, D. L., Tzeng,
O. J., Muggleton, N. G., et al. (2010). Anti-saccade
cost is modulated by contextual experience of location
probability. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(3), 1438-
1447.
Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Munoz, D. P., Merriam, E. P.,
Garver, K. E., Minshew, N. J., et al. (2001).
Maturation of widely distributed brain function sub-
serves cognitive development. Neuroimage, 13(5), 786-
793.
Mahone, E. M., Mostofsky, S. H., Lasker, A. G., Zee, D., &
Denckla, M. B. (2009). Oculomotor anomalies in
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for
deficits in response preparation and inhibition.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(7), 749-756.
Massen, C. (2004). Parallel programming of exogenous and
endogenous components in the anti-saccade task. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3),
475-498.
McNamee, R. L., Dunfee, K. L., Luna, B., Clark, D. B.,
Eddy, W. F., & Tarter, R. E. (2008). Brain activation,
response inhibition, and increased risk for substance
use disorder. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
Research, 32(3), 405-413.
Michel, F., & Anderson, M. (2009). Using the anti-saccade
task to investigate the relationship between the
development of inhibition and the development of
intelligence. Developmental Science, 12(2), 272-288.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A.
H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and
diversity of executive functions and their
contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: A
latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1),
49-100.
Mokler, A., & Fischer, B. (1999). The recognition and
correction of involuntary pro-saccades in an anti-
saccade task. Experimental Brain Research, 125(4), 511-
516.
Munoz, D. P., Armstrong, I. T., Hampton, K. A., & Moore,
K. D. (2003). Altered control of visual fixation and
saccadic eye movements in attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90
(1), 503-514.
Munoz, D. P., Broughton, J. R., Goldring, J. E., &
Armstrong, I. T. (1998). Age-related performance of
human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks.
Experimental Brain Research, 121, 391-400.
Munoz, D. P., & Everling, S. (2004). Look away: The anti-
saccade task and the voluntary control of eye
movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(3), 218-228.
Nakagawa, A., & Sukigara, M. (2007). Infant eye and head
movements toward the side opposite the cue in the anti-
saccade paradigm. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(5).
O'Driscoll, G. A., Alpert, N. M., Matthysse, S. W., Levy,
D. L., Rauch, S. L., & Holzman, P. S. (1995).
Functional neuroanatomy of anti-saccade eye movements
investigated with positron emission tomography.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America, 92(3), 925-929.
O'Driscoll, G. A., Depatie, L., Holahan, A. L., Savion-
Lemieux, T., Barr, R. G., Jolicoeur, C., et al.
(2005). Executive functions and methylphenidate
response in subtypes of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry,
57(11), 1452-1460.
Olk, B., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Why are anti-saccades
slower than pro-saccades? A novel finding using a new
paradigm. NeuroReport, 14(1), 151-155.
Park, J., Shimojo, E., & Shimojo, S. (2010). Roles of
familiarity and novelty in visual preference judgments
are segregated across object categories. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 107(33), 14552-14555.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3-
25.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1980). The development
of attentional mechanisms. Nebraska Symposium on
Motivation, 28, 1-52.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Educating the
human brain. Washington DC:American Psychological
Association.
Reuter, B., Rakusan, L., & Kathmanna, N. (2005). Poor anti-
saccade performance in schizophrenia: An inhibition
deficit? Psychiatry Research, 135(1), 1-10.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D.,
Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., et al. (2004).
Development of attentional networks in childhood.
Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029-1040.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Davis-
Stober, C. P. (2004). Development of the time course
for processing conflict: An event-related potentials
study with 4 year olds and adults. BMC neuroscience,
5, 39.
Scerif, G., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Campos, R., Elsabbagh,
M., Driver, J., & Cornish, K. (2005). To look or not
to look? Typical and atypical development of
oculomotor control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
17(4), 591-604.
Smyrnis, N., Evdokimidis, I., Stefanis, N. C.,
Constantinidis, T. S., Avramopoulos, D., Theleritis,
C., et al. (2002). The anti-saccade task in a sample
of 2,006 young males. II. Effects of task parameters.
Experimental Brain Research, 147(1), 53-63.
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome,
S. E., Kan, E., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Developmental
mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in
normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(38), 8223-
8231.
St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006).
Executive functions and achievements in school:
Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory.
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59
(4), 745-759.
Thatcher, R., Walker, R., & Giudice, S. (1987). Human
cerebral hemispheres develop at different rates and
ages. Science, 236(4805), 1110-1113.
Thatcher, R. W., North, D. M., & Biver, C. J. (2008).
Development of cortical connections as measured by EEG
coherence and phase delays. Human Brain Mapping, 29
(12), 1400-1415.
Tillman, C. M., Thorell, L. B., Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin,
G. (2008). Motor response inhibition and execution in
the stop-signal task: Development and relation to ADHD
behaviors. Child Neuropsychology, 14(1), 42-59.
Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). The stochastic
modeling of elementary psychological processes.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Towse, J. N., Lewis, C., & Knowles, M. (2007). When
knowledge is not enough: The phenomenon of goal
neglect in preschool children. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 96(4), 320-332.
Towse, J. N., Redbond, J., Houston-Price, C. M. T., &
Cook, S. (2000). Understanding the dimensional change
card sort: Perspectives from task success and failure.
Cognitive Development, 15(3), 347-365.
|