博碩士論文 104825008 詳細資訊




以作者查詢圖書館館藏 以作者查詢臺灣博碩士 以作者查詢全國書目 勘誤回報 、線上人數:26 、訪客IP:3.145.94.164
姓名 黃義勝(Yi-Sheng Wong)  查詢紙本館藏   畢業系所 認知與神經科學研究所
論文名稱 Do Erotic Stimuli Interfere with Response Inhibition? A Comparison of EEG between Sexual Offenders and Non-Offender Controls
(Do Erotic Stimuli Interfere with Response Inhibition? A Comparison of EEG between Sexual Offenders and Non-Offender Controls)
檔案 [Endnote RIS 格式]    [Bibtex 格式]    [相關文章]   [文章引用]   [完整記錄]   [館藏目錄]   [檢視]  [下載]
  1. 本電子論文使用權限為同意立即開放。
  2. 已達開放權限電子全文僅授權使用者為學術研究之目的,進行個人非營利性質之檢索、閱讀、列印。
  3. 請遵守中華民國著作權法之相關規定,切勿任意重製、散佈、改作、轉貼、播送,以免觸法。

摘要(中) 本論文研究著重在利用情緒性停止訊號作業來了解情緒對行為抑制的影響。在受試者進行停止訊號作業的同時也量測他們的腦電波以便了解情緒性刺激對認知功能的影響。本研究招收了兩組對象:性侵犯組與年齡匹配非罪犯組。受試者若在觀看色情圖片或中性圖片後聽到“嗶”聲,即必須抑制按鍵反應。研究結果顯示,不論出現的圖片為情緒性或中性,兩組在停止信號延遲時間 (SSD) 上有顯著的差異。色情圖片和中性圖片之間的停止信號反應時間 (SSRT) 差異在性侵犯組達到邊緣顯著。和控制組相比,性侵犯組的表現只有在觀看了色情圖片後才受到影響。對比色情圖片減去中性圖片的情況,性侵犯組顯示出更強的delta (0.5-1 Hz) 和theta波 (4-8 Hz),而控制組只顯示出更強的delta波 (0.5-1 Hz)。對比成功抑制反應減去失敗抑制反應,色情圖片出現且受試者成功抑制反應減去色情圖片出現但受試者抑制反應失敗,以及中性圖片出現且受試者成功抑制反應減去中性圖片出現但受試者抑制反應失敗的情況,兩組都顯示出更強的delta波 (1-2 Hz) 和更低的theta波 (4-8 Hz)。兩組的腦電波在所有對比中都無顯著差異。這些研究結果顯示情緒性刺激對這兩組實驗對象有著不同的影響,以及性侵犯可能有較差的抑制控制能力。
摘要(英) The present study aimed to investigate the effect of emotion on response inhibition by applying an emotional stop-signal task. Concurrent recording of the electroencephalogram (EEG) was applied to allow measurement of the influence of emotional content on cognitive functions. A group of sexual offenders and age-matched non-offender controls were recruited. Erotic and neutral pictures were shown shortly before the appearance of an auditory stop-signal, which required action cancellation. The result showed a significant difference in stop-signal delay between the two groups in both picture conditions. Marginal significant between erotic and neutral stop-signal reaction time was revealed for sexual offenders. Compared to the controls, the performance of the sexual offenders was only impaired in the erotic picture condition. Greater power of delta oscillations (0.5-1 Hz) and theta oscillations (4-8 Hz) in sexual offenders and greater power of delta oscillations (0.5-1 Hz) in controls was revealed in the contrast erotic picture minus neutral picture trials. Both groups showed greater power of delta oscillations (1-2 Hz) and lower power of theta oscillations (4-8 Hz) in the contrast successful minus unsuccessful stop trials, erotic successful minus erotic unsuccessful stop trials, and neutral successful minus neutral unsuccessful stop trials. No significant EEG difference was found between the two groups on all comparison. These findings suggest that emotional stimuli may be processed with different weights between the two groups and sexual offenders might have problems with inhibitory control.
關鍵字(中) ★ 情緒性停止訊號作業
★ 抑制控制
★ 情色刺激
★ 性侵犯
★ 腦電波
關鍵字(英) ★ emotional stop-signal task
★ inhibitory control
★ erotic stimuli
★ sexual offender
★ electroencephalogram (EEG)
論文目次 Chinese Abstract i
Abstract ii
Table of Content iii
Table of Figures vi
Chapter 1. General Introduction 1
1.1 Introduction 1
1.2 Theories of sexual offending 2
1.2.1 The Precondition Model 2
1.2.2 The Integrated Theory of Sexual Offending 3
1.3 Characteristic of rapists 5
1.4 Inhibitory control 7
1.5 Stop-signal task 8
1.6 The inhibitory control tasks using offender samples 10
1.7 Cortical mechanisms of response inhibition 11
1.8 Effect of emotional stimulus on response inhibition 15
1.9 Neural mechanisms of emotional stimulus 16
1.10 Purpose and hypothesis of the current study 17
Chapter 2. Materials and Methods 19
2.1 Participants 19
2.2 Apparatus, stimuli, task, procedure, and measures 19
2.2.1 Apparatus 19
2.2.2 Pictorial stimuli 20
2.2.3 Task 20
2.2.4 Procedure 22
2.2.5 Measures 23
2.2.5.1 Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices 23
2.2.5.2 Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 24
2.2.5.3 Negative Mood Regulation 24
2.3 EEG Acquisition 25
2.4 Data Analysis 25
2.4.1 Behavioral data 25
2.4.2 EEG Preprocessing 26
2.4.3 Hilbert-Huang Transformation 27
2.5 Statistical assessment 28
2.5.1 Behavioral data 28
2.5.2 EEG data 29
Chapter 3. Results 31
3.1 Behavioral data 31
3.1.1 Sexual Offender vs. Control group 31
3.1.2 Sexual offender group 33
3.1.3 Control group 33
3.2 Measures 34
3.2.1 Sexual offender group 34
3.2.2 Control group 34
3.2.3 Sexual offender vs. Control group 35
3.3 EEG 35
3.3.1 EEG time-locked to picture presentation 35
3.3.1.1 Sexual offender group 35
3.3.1.2 Control group 36
3.3.1.3 Sexual offender group vs. Control group 37
3.3.2 EEG time-locked to stop-signal presentation 37
3.3.2.1 Sexual offender group 37
3.3.2.2 Control group 41
3.3.2.3 Sexual offender group vs. Control group 45
Chapter 4. Discussion 46
4.1 Measures 46
4.2 Behavioral results 47
4.3 EEG results 48
4.3.1 EEG analysis time-locked to picture presentation 48
4.3.2 EEG analysis time-locked to stop-signal presentation 50
4.4 Limitations and future directions 52
Chapter 5. Conclusion 53
References 54
Appendix 1 70
Appendix 2 71
Appendix 3 72
Appendix 4 73
參考文獻 Adjorlolo, S., & Egbenya, L. D. (2016). Executive functioning profiles of adult and juvenile male sexual offenders: A systematic review. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 27(3), 349-375.
Aftanas, L. I., Reva, N. V., & Makhnev, V. P. (2008). Individual variability of brain oscillatory and autonomous concomitants of motivated attention, International journal of psychophysiology, 69, 197. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.05.533
Argiolas, A., & Melis, M. R. (2005). Central control of penile erection: role of the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. Progress in Neurobiology, 76(1), 1-21. Doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.06.002
Armstrong, I. T., & Munoz, D. P. (2003). Inhibitory control of eye movements during oculomotor countermanding in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Exp Brain Res, 152(4), 444-452.
Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 55-68.
Aron, A. R., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J. Neurosci., 26, 2424-2433. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006
Baker, L. R. (1999). What is this thing called commonsense psychology? Philosophical Explorations, 2, 3-19.
Baker, L. R. (2012). From consciousness to self-consciousness. Grazer Philosophische Studien, 84, 19-38.
Balconi, M., & Pozzoli, U. (2008). Event-related oscillations (ERO) and event-related potentials (ERP) in emotional face recognition. International journal of neuroscience, 118(10), 1412-1424.
Band, G. P., van der Molen, M. W., & Logan, G. D. (2003). Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. Acta Psychol., 112, 105–142.
Bard, L., Carter, D., Cerce, D., Knight, R., Rosenberg, R., & Schneider, B. (1987). A descriptive study of rapists and child molesters. Developmental, clinical and criminal characteristics. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 5, 203−220.
Bari, A., & Robbins, W. T. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology, 108, 44-79.
Başar, E. (1998). Brain Function and Oscillations. Vol 1: Brain Oscillations, Principles and Approaches. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 363.
Bekkedal, M. Y., Rossi, J. 3rd, & Panksepp, J. (2011). Human brain EEG indices of emotions: delineating responses to affective vocalizations by measuring frontal theta event-related synchronization. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(9), 1959-1970. Doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.001
Berkowitz, A. (1992). College men as perpetrators of acquaintance rape and sexual assault: A review of recent research. College Health, 40, 175–181.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433-436.
Bruchmann, M., Herper, K., Konrad, C., Pantev, C., & Huster, R. J. (2010). Individualized EEG source reconstruction of Stroop interference with masked color words. Neuroimage, 49, 1800-1809.
Burt, M. R. (1980). Cultural myths and supports for rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 217-130.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 191–214.
Catanzaro, S. J., & Mearns, J. (1990). Measuring generalized expectancies for negative mood regulation: initial scale development and implications. Journal of personality assessment, 54(3&4), 546-563.
Cavanagh, J. F. & Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 414-421.
Chamberlain, S. R., Blackwell, A. D., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005). The neuropsychology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 29(3), 399-419.
Chen, C. Y., Muggleton, N. G., Juan, C. H., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hung, D. L. (2008). Time pressure leads to inhibitory control deficits in impulsive violent offenders. Behavioural brain research, 187(2), 483-488.
Chevrier, A. D., Noseworthy, M. D., & Schachar, R. (2007). Dissociation of response inhibition and performance monitoring in the stop signal task using event-related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp, 28(12), 1347-1358.
Craig, M. E. (1990). Coercive sexuality in dating relationships: A situational model. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 395–423.
Craissati, J. (2005). Sexual violence against women: A psychological approach to the assessment and management of rapists in the community. Probation Journal: The Journal of Community and Criminal Justice, 52, 401–422.
Crowell, N. A., & Burgess, A. W. (1996). Understanding violence against women. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.


Department of Protective Services, Ministry of Health and Welfare. (2017). Statistics of Reporting Cases of Sexual Assault. Retrieved online: http://www.mohw.gov.tw/cht/DOPS/DM1.aspx?f_list_no=806&fod_list_no=4622 [Access April 2017].
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168.
Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2000).Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23, 475-483.
Duque, J., Greenhouse, I., Labruna, L., & Ivry, R. B. (2017). Physiological markers of motor inhibition during human behavior. Trends in neurosciences, 40(4), 219-236. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.02.006.
Elchlepp, H., & Verbruggen, F. (2017). How to withhold or replace a prepotent response: An analysis of the underlying control processes and their temporal dynamics. Biological Psychology, 123, 250-268.
Engel, A. K., & Fries, P. (2010). Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 156-165.
Feng, C., Wang, L., Wang, N., Gu, R., & Luo, Y. J. (2012). The time course of implicit processing of erotic pictures: an event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1489, 48-55.
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and research. New York, NY: Free Press.
Galashan, D., Wittfoth, M., Fehr, T., & Herrmann, M. (2008). Two Simon tasks with different sources of conflict: an ERP study of motion- and location-based compatibility effects. Biological Psychology, 78(3), 246-252.
Glenn, A. L., & Raine, A. (2014). Neurocriminology: implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behavior. Nature Review Neuroscience, 15, 54-63.
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A General Theory of Crime. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Gray, J. A. (1999). Cognition, emotion, conscious experience and the brain. In: Dalgleish, T., Power, M. (Eds.), Handbook of Cognition and Emotion. John Wiley and Sons Ltd..
Greenhouse, I., Swann, N. C., & Aron, A. R. (2012). Fronto-Basal-Ganglia Circuits for Stopping Action. In Mars, R. B., Sallet, J., Rushworth, M. F. S., & Yeung, N. (Eds.). Neural basis of motivational and cognitive control. (pp. 189-207). The MIT press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, London, England.
Groth, A. N. (1979). Sexual trauma in the life histories of rapists and child molesters. Victimology: An International Journal, 4, 10–16.
Groth, A. N., & Birnbaum, A. H. (1979). Men who rape: The psychology of the offender. New York: Plenum.
Groth, A. N., & Burgess, A. W. (1970). Rape: A sexual deviation. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 47, 400–406.
Groth, A. N., & Burgess, A. W. (1978). Rape: A pseudosexual act. International Journal of Women′s Studies, 1, 207–210.
Groth, A. N., & Oliveri, F. (1989). Understanding sexual abuse behavior and differentiating among sexual abusers. In S. Sgroi (Ed.), Vulnerable populations, Vol. 2. (pp. 309–327). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Groth, A. N., Burgess, A. W., & Holmstrom, L. L. (1977). Rape: Power, anger, and sexuality. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1239–1243.
Hanes, D. P., & Schall, J. D. (1996). Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science, 274(5286), 427-430.
Hayashi, T., Mizuno-Matsumoto, Y., Kohri, S., Nitta, Y., & Tonoike, M. (2016). Event-related theta and alpha oscillations under emotional stimuli: an MEG study. Intelligent automation & soft computing, 23(1), 175-182.
Health, R. G. (1972). Pleasure and brain activity in man. Deep surface electroencephalograms during orgasm. Journal of nervous and mental disease, 154(1), 3-18.
Hsu, T. Y., Tseng, L. Y., Yu, J. X., Kuo, W. J., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Juan, C. H. (2011). Modulating inhibitory control with direct current stimulation of the superior medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage, 56(4), 2249–2257.
Huang, N. E., Long, S. R., & Shen, Z. (1996).The mechanism for frequency downshift in nonlinear wave evolution. Advances in Applied Mechanics, 32, 59-117.
Huang, N. E., Shen, Z., Long, S. R., Wu, M. L. C., Shih, H. H., Zheng, Q., Yen, N. C., Tung, C. C., & Liu, H. H. (1998). The empirical mode decomposition and the Hilbert spectrum for nonlinear and non-stationary time series analysis. Proceedings of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, physical and engineering sciences, 454(1971), 903-995. doi:10.1098/rspa.1998.0193
Hunter, J. A., Hazelwood, R. R., & Slesinger, D. (2000). Juvenile-perpetrated sex crimes: Patterns of offending and predictors of violence. Journal of Family Violence, 15, 81−93.
Huster, R. J., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Lavallee, C. F., Falkenstein, M., & Herrmann, C. S. (2013). Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: Functional networks and cognitive contributions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87, 217–233.
Jensen, O., & Colgin, L. L. (2007). Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 267-269.
Juan, C. L., & Muggleton, N. G. (2012). Brain stimulation and inhibitory control. Brain Stimulation, 5, 63-69.
Kalanthroff, E., Cohen, N., & Henik, A. (2013). Stop feeling: inhibition of emotional interference following stop-signal trials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(78), 1-7. Doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00078
Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2007). Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biol Psychiatry, 62(3), 198-206.
Kenemans, J. L. (2015). Specific proactive and generic reactive inhibition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 56, 115-126.
Khng, K. H., & Lee, K. (2014). The relationship between Stroop and stop-signal measures of inhibition in adolescents: influences from variations in context and measure estimation. PLoS ONE, 9, e101356.
Kim, S. W., Sohn, D. W., Cho, Y. H., Yang, W. S., Lee, K. U., Juh, R., Ahn, K. J., Chung, Y. A., Han, S. I., Lee, K. H., Lee, C. U., & Chae, J. H. (2006). Brain activation by visual erotic stimuli in healthy middle aged males. International Journal of Impotence Research, 18, 452-457.

Klados, M. A., Frantzidis, C., Vivas, A. B., Papadelis, C., Lithari, C., Pappas, C. & Bamidis, P. D. (2009). A framework combining delta event-related oscillations (EROs) and synchronization effects (ERD/ERS) to study emotional processing. Computational intelligence and neuroscience, 2009: 549419, 1-16.
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36, ECVP Abstract Supplement.
Klimesch, W., Hanslmayr, S., Sauseng, P., Gruber, W., Brozinsky, C. J., Kroll, N. E. A., Yonelinas, A. P., & Doppelmayr, M. (2006). Oscillatory EEG correlates of episodic trace decay. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 280-290.
Knyazev, G. G. (2007). Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 377-395.
Knyazev, G. G. (2012). EEG delta oscillations as a correlate of basic homeostatic and motivational processes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 677-695.
Knyazev, G. G., Slobodskoj-Plusnin, J. Y., & Bocharov, A. V. (2009). Event-related delta and theta synchronization during explicit and implicit emotion processing. Neuroscience 164, 1588-1600.doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.057
Ko, L. W., Shih, Y. C., Chikara, R. K., Chuang, Y. T., & Chang, E. C. (2016). Neural mechanisms of inhibitory response in a battlefield scenario: a simultaneous fMRI-EEG study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10(185), 1-12.
Kok, A., Ramautar, J. R., De Ruiter, M. B., Band, G. P., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2004). ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal task. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 9-20.
Kopp, B., Rist, F., & Mattler, U. (1996). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology, 33, 282-294.
Koss, M. P., Leonard, K., Beezley, D., & Oros, C. J. (1985). Non-stranger sexual aggression: A discriminant analysis of the psychological characteristics of undetected offenders. Sex Roles, 12, 981–992.
Krämer, U. M., Knight, R. T., & Münte, T. F. (2011). Electrophysiological evidence for different inhibitory mechanisms when stopping or changing a planned response. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2481-2493.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
LǺngström, N., Sjöstedt, G., & Grann, M. (2004). Psychiatric disorders and recidivism in sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 16(2), 139-150.
Lavallee, C. F., Herrmann, C. S., Weerda, R., & Huster, R. J. (2014). Stimulus-Response mappings shape inhibition processes: a combined EEG-fMRI study of contextual stopping. PLoS ONE, 9(4): e96159.
Li, C. S. R., Chang, H. L., Hsu, Y. P., Wang, H. S., & Ko, N. C. ( 2006). Motor response inhibition in children with Tourette′ s disorder. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 18, 417-419.
Li, C. S. R., Huang, C., Constable, R. T., & Sinha, R. (2006). Imaging response inhibition in a stop-signal task: neural correlates independent of signal monitoring and post-response processing. J Neurosci, 26, 186-192.
Logan, G. D., & Cowan,W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91, 295–327.
Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychol Sci, 8(1), 60-64.
Luria, A. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Malamuth, N. M. (1986). Predictors of naturalistic sexual aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 953–962.
Meijers, J., Harte, J. M., Meynen, G. & Cuijpers, P. (2017). Differences in executive functioning between violent and non-violent offenders. Psychological Medicine, 47(10), 1784-1793.
Miller, L. (2000). The predator′s brain: Neuro-psychodynamics of serial killers. In L. B. Schlesinger (Ed.), Serial offenders: Current thought, recent findings, unusual syndromes (pp. 135–166). Boca Raton: CRC Press.
Miller, L. (2014). Rape: sex crime, act of violence, or naturalistic adaptation? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19, 67-81.
Mizuseki, K., Sirota, A., Pastalkova, E., & Buzsáki, G. (2009). Theta oscillations provide temporal windows for local circuit computation in the entorhinal-hippocampal loop. Neuron, 64, 267-280.
Mostofsky, S. H., Schafer, J. G., Abrams, M. T., Goldberg, M. C., Flower, A. A., Boyce, A., Courtney, S. M., Calhoun, V. D., Kraut, M. A., Denckla, M. B., & Pekar, J. J. (2003). fMRI evidence that the neural basis of response inhibition is task-dependent. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 419-430.
Osterburg, W. J. & Ward, H. R. (2010). Criminal investigation: a method for reconstructing the past, 6th edtion. Southington: Anderson Publishing.
Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, Journal of clinical psychology, 51(6), 768-774.
Pennington, B. F. (2002). The development of psychopathology: Nature and nurture. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 188-196.
Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 160-166.
Pittman-Polletta, B., Hsieh, W. H., Kaur, S., Lo, M. T., & Hu. K. (2014). Detecting Phase-Amplitude Coupling with High Frequency Resolution using Adaptive Decompositions. Journal of neuroscience methods, 226, 15-32.
Polaschek, D. L. L., Ward, T., & Hudson, S. M. (1997). Rape and rapists: Theory and treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 117–144.
Price, S. A., & Hanson, R. K. (2007). A modified Stroop task with sexual offenders: Replication of a study, Journal of Sexual Aggression, 13(3), 203-216.
Price, S. A., Beech, A. R., Mitchell, I., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). Measuring deviant sexual interest in adolescents using the emotional Stroop task. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 26(5), 450-471.
Ramautar, J. R., Kok, A., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2006). Effects of stop-signal modality on the N2/P3 complex elicited in the stop-signal paradigm. Biological Psychology, 72, 96–109.
Randall, W. M., & Smith, J. L. (2011). Conflict and inhibition in the cued-Go/NoGo task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122(12), 2400–2407.
Rangel-Gomez, M., Knight, R. T., & Krämer, U. M. (2015). How to stop or change a motor response: Laplacian and independent component analysis approach. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97, 233–244.
Raven, J. (1990). Advanced Progressive Matrices: Sets I, II. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 1-48. Doi: 10.1003/cogp.1999.0735
Sauseng, P., Griesmayr, B., Freunberger, R., & Klimesch, W. (2010). Control mechanisms in working memory: a possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 1015-1022.
Schachar, R., & Logan, G. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Dev Psychol, 26, 710-720.
Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (1995). Test of four hypotheses for the comorbidity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 639-648.
Schachar, R., Tannock, R., & Logan, G. D. (1993). Inhibitory control, impulsiveness, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin Psychol Rev, 13(8), 721-739.
Schachar, R., Tannock, R., Marriott, M., & Logan, G. (1995). Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 23(4), 411-437.
Schmiedt-Fehr, C., Dühl, S., & Basar-Eroglu, C. (2011). Age-related increases in within person variability: delta and theta oscillations indicate that the elderly are not always old. Neuroscience Letters, 495, 159-163.
Scully, D. (1990). Understanding sexual violence: a study of convicted rapists. Boston: Unwin.
Senderecka, M. (2016). Threatening visual stimuli influence response inhibition and error monitoring: an event-related potential study. Biological Psychology, 113, 24-36.
Seo, Y., Jeong, B., Kim, J. W., & Choi, J. (2010). The relationship between age and brain response to visual erotic stimuli in healthy heterosexual males. International journal of impotence research, 22, 234-239.
Simmonds, D. J., Pekar, J. J., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2008). Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task dependent. Neuropsychologia, 46, 224-232.
Simon, L. M. J. (2000). An examination of the assumptions of specialization, mental disorder, and dangerousness in sex offenders. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 18, 275−308.
Simons, D. A. (2014). Sex Offender Typologies. Sex offender management assessment and planning initiative. U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Office of Sex Offender Sentencing, Monitoring, Apprehending, Registering, and Tracking. Retrieved online from: https://ojp.gov/smart/SOMAPI/pdfs/SOMAPI_Full%20Report.pdf [Accessed April 2017].
Smith, P. & Waterman, M. (2004). Processing bias for sexual material: The emotional Stroop and sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 16, 163-171.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.
Swann, N. C., Cai, W., Conner, C. R., Pieters, T. A., Claffey, M. P., George, J. S., Aron, A. R., & Tandon, N. (2012). Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity. NeuroImage, 59, 2860-2870.



Swann, N. C., Tandon, N., Canolty, R., Ellmore, T. M., McEvoy, L. K., Dreyer, S., DiSano, M., & Aron, A. R. (2009). Intracranial EEG reveals a time- and frequency-specific role for the right inferior frontal gyrus and primary motor cortex in stopping initiated responses. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(40), 12675-12685.
van der Schoot, M., Licht, R., Horsley, T. M., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Effects of stop signal modality, stop signal intensity and tracking method on inhibitory performance as determined by use of the stop signal paradigm. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 331–341.
van Lankveld, J. J. D. M., & Smulders, F. T. Y. (2008). The effect of visual sexual content on the event-related potential. Biological Psychology, 79, 200-208.
Verbruggen, F., & De Houwer, J. (2007). Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 391-403. doi:10.1080/02699930600625081
Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 647-661. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev .2008.08.014
Walmsley, R. (2016). World Prison Population List, 11th edition. Core Publications. Retrieved online at: http://www.prisonstudies.org/sites/default/files/resources/downloads/world_prison_population_list_11th_edition_0.pdf [Accessed April 2017].
Wang, C. H., Chang, C. C., Liang, Y. M., Shih, C. M., Chiu, W. S., Tseng, P., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J. L., Muggleton, N. G., & Juan, C. H. (2013). Open vs. closed skill sports and the modulation of inhibitory control. PLOS ONE 8(2): e55773.
Ward, T., & Beech, A. R. (2006). An integrated theory of sexual offending. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 11, 44-63.
Ward, T., & Beech, A. R. (2016). The integrated theory of sexual offending – revised: a multifield perspective. In: Boer, D. P. (Ed.), The Wiley Handbook on the Theories, Assessment, and Treatment of Sexual Offending, Volume I: Theories. (p. 123 – 137). Wiley-Blackwell.
Ward, T., & Hudson, S. M. (2000). A self-regulation model of relapse prevention. In D. R. Laws, S. M. Hudson, & T. Ward (Eds.), Remaking relapse prevention with sex offenders: A sourcebook (pp. 79–101). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ward, T., & Hudson, S. M. (2001). Finkelhor′s precondition model of child sexual abuse: A critique. Psychology, Crime & Law, 7, 291-307.
Wessel, J. R., & Aron, A. R. (2013). Unexpected events induce motor slowing via a brain mechanism for action-stopping with global suppressive effects. Journal of neuroscience, 33(47), 18481-18491.
Wessel, J. R., & Aron, A. R. (2017). On the globality of motor suppression: unexpected events and their influence on behavior and cognition. Neuron, 93, 259-280.
White, D. J., Congedo, M., Ciorciari, J., & Silberstein, R. B. (2011). Brain oscillatory activity during spatial navigation: theta and gamma activity link medial temporal and parietal regions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 686-697.
Wickens, C. D., & Kessel, C. (1980). Processing resource demands of failure detection in dynamic systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 6, 564–577.

World Health Organization (2012a). Mental health and psychosocial support for conflict-related sexual violence: Principles and interventions. Retrieved online: http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/75179/1/WHO_RHR_HRP_12.18_eng.pdf
World Health Organization (2012b). Mental health and psychosocial support for conflict-related sexual violence: 10 myths. Retrieved online: http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/75177/1/WHO_RHR_HRP_12.17_eng.pdf
Wu, Z., & Huang, N. E. (2009) Ensemble empirical mode decomposition: a noise-assisted data analysis method. Advances in Adaptive Data Analysis, 1, 1-41.
Yu, J. Tseng, P., Muggleton, N. G., & Juan, C. H. (2015). Being watched by others eliminates the effect of emotional arousal on inhibitory control. Frontiers in psychology, 6(4), 1-5.
Yu, J., Hung, D. L., Tseng, P., Tzeng, O. J. L., Muggleton, N. G., & Juan, C. J. (2012). Sex differences in how erotic and painful stimuli impair inhibitory control. Cognition, 124, 252-255.
指導教授 陳永儀 梁偉光(Yung-Yee Chen Wei-Kuang Liang) 審核日期 2017-10-5
推文 facebook   plurk   twitter   funp   google   live   udn   HD   myshare   reddit   netvibes   friend   youpush   delicious   baidu   
網路書籤 Google bookmarks   del.icio.us   hemidemi   myshare   

若有論文相關問題,請聯絡國立中央大學圖書館推廣服務組 TEL:(03)422-7151轉57407,或E-mail聯絡  - 隱私權政策聲明