博碩士論文 942207006 詳細資訊




以作者查詢圖書館館藏 以作者查詢臺灣博碩士 以作者查詢全國書目 勘誤回報 、線上人數:24 、訪客IP:18.119.104.238
姓名 周毓瑩(Yu-ying Chou)  查詢紙本館藏   畢業系所 認知與神經科學研究所
論文名稱 運用系列情緒史楚普作業探討性侵害犯及正常控制組在情緒及認知上之交互運作:事件相關腦電位研究
(Using Serial Emotional Stroop Tasks to Examine the Emotion-Cognition Interactions in the Sex Offenders and the Healthy Controls)
相關論文
★ 探索電玩遊戲頻率對於視覺注意力表現能力的效應★ 表音一致性、聲旁結合度對中文閱讀的影響
★ 神經膠細胞在神經同步活動及鈣離子波傳遞中之角色★ 性侵害犯的衝動行為表現-情緒狀態如何影響性侵害犯的抑制能力?
★ 思考死亡對於說謊行為的調節效果
檔案 [Endnote RIS 格式]    [Bibtex 格式]    [相關文章]   [文章引用]   [完整記錄]   [館藏目錄]   [檢視]  [下載]
  1. 本電子論文使用權限為同意立即開放。
  2. 已達開放權限電子全文僅授權使用者為學術研究之目的,進行個人非營利性質之檢索、閱讀、列印。
  3. 請遵守中華民國著作權法之相關規定,切勿任意重製、散佈、改作、轉貼、播送,以免觸法。

摘要(中) 本研究運用多個情緒性史楚普作業(emotional Stroop task)探討選擇性注意力如何被不同情緒的刺激所影響,且藉由事件相關腦電位(Event-Related Potentials)的研究方法,進一步地檢驗連續性侵害犯是否較易被煽情刺激吸引,試圖找出性侵犯罪行為與大腦神經機制之間的關係。在情緒性史楚普作業中,受試者需忽略字詞的意義、儘快且正確地對字詞的墨水顏色做出反應。過去的研究結果發現:受試者對情緒字詞唸色的反應時間會比中性字詞的反應時間慢,此現象稱作情緒的史楚普干擾效應(Stroop interference effect)。一般認為此效應主要是因為:對不同情緒類別的字詞,受試者有不同的注意力分配。對情緒愈鮮明的字詞,我們愈會有注意力偏向(attentional bias)。本研究共進行了三個實驗來檢視正常控制組及性侵害犯受到不同情緒刺激的干擾程度,並加以探討兩組受試者上情緒和認知交互運作的情形。
實驗一及實驗2-1為行為實驗,分別使用口語和按鍵作為反應方式,主要對象為一般男性大學生,從反應時間的結果上發現:對於煽情字詞的反應時間顯著慢於中性字詞,顯示受試者會不自覺地受到煽情字詞的干擾,也就是將較多的注意力資源放在處理煽情字詞上。
實驗2-2進一步地紀錄及比較一般男性大學生在看到中性及煽情字詞時所伴隨發生的事件相關腦電位(ERPs),結果顯示無論是在早期或晚期的腦波成分上,都可看到受試者對於煽情字詞投入較多的注意力資源,尤其晚期的正向腦波(late positive potentials)更跟行為反應上的注意力偏向有顯著的正相關。實驗2-3將實驗2-2應用在連續性侵害犯上,結果發現連續性侵害犯跟正常控制組一樣對於煽情刺激會投入較多的注意力,但跟行為上的注意力偏向最有相關的腦波成分則是早期的N200,此結果顯示連續性侵害犯在處理這些煽情字詞的認知歷程上異於一般男性大學生。
實驗三將實驗刺激由文字改為圖片,我們依然操弄刺激的情緒類別,並且給予受試者反應上的時間壓力以避免可能的策略運用,企圖找出兩組受試者在處理煽情刺激時的反應時間及腦波振幅上是否有所不同。實驗3-1使用加上外框的彩色圖片,要求受試者忽略圖片意義,只對外框顏色做按鍵反應;結果發現負向及煽情圖片反應時間顯著慢於中性,但在控制實驗中也找到負向圖片慢於中性圖片的結果,代表顏色這類基本的知覺屬性仍會是實驗的干擾變項,因此在實驗3-2中我們將所有圖片以灰階的形式呈現,且同時紀錄受試者的腦波變化。結果指出,在正常控制組及連續性侵害犯的行為結果上都沒有情緒的主要效果;但在ERPs結果上,不論在早期(N200)或晚期(LPP)成分上,都可看到在煽情圖片情況下兩組受試者投注較多的注意力資源。更重要的,我們發現:這兩組受試者可能運用了不同的神經機制來處理這項作業,也就是在應對情緒會影響認知的情況下,兩組受試者有著不同型態的認知資源分配。
從這一系列情緒性史楚普作業中,我們發現正常控制組及連續性侵害犯在情緒效果上無明顯「量」上的不同;有趣的是,從腦波分布、相關上看到他們有「質」上的不同,因此我們就幾個可能因素做深入討論。
最後,關於未來的研究方向,我們試圖採用衝突更大的情緒史楚普作業,專門探討情緒上的衝突,進而檢驗在衝突監控、解決上,一般男性大學生和連續性侵害犯是否會有差異。
摘要(英) In the current study, a series of emotional Stroop tasks were used to examine the emotion-cognition interactions across the sex offenders and the normal controls. According to the previous research, the individuals with various emotional and drug-abuse disorders demonstrated attentional bias for the stimuli specifically related to their characteristics. Therefore, we employed the emotional Stroop paradigm including both versions of words and pictures (Exp. 2 & Exp. 3) to investigate whether the sex offenders showed the differential attentional bias effect compared to the controls while their brain potentials were measured.
In the experiment 2, we found the longer reaction time for the erotic words both in the sex offenders and the normal controls. The ERP results showed that the main effects of emotion on the P2, N2 and late positive potential (LPP) in the sex offenders while on the N2 and LPP in the controls. More interesting, due to the marginally significant interaction effect of emotion and group on the LPP component, this indicated the sex offenders seemed to pay less sustained-attention to the erotic words compared to the controls. We suggest that the sex offenders might use the deliberate regulation of the erotic influence. Furthermore, the positive relations between the LPP and the bias scores were significant in the controls. However, instead of the LPP, the N2 component positively correlated with the bias scores in the sex offenders.
In the experiment 3, behaviorally, there was no main effect of emotion in both groups, but the ERP results revealed that there were significant modulations of emotion on the early and late components, the N2 and LPP, in both groups. Furthermore, the differential scalp distribution patterns of emotional modulations on the two groups were found, that was, we found a significant three-way interaction effect (Caudality × Emotion × Group).
In summary, the current study demonstrated that the attentional bias effect occurred not only behaviorally but also in our brains. Namely, we found the emotional modulations on the ERP components. These indicate that the capacity-limited attention resource was voluntarily devoted to the emotionally salient events. More important, the different performances between the sex offenders and the controls may imply that there were differential neural mechanisms underlying the emotion-cognition interactions between the two groups. Further, the possible causes, implications, and directions for future research are discussed.
關鍵字(中) ★ 事件相關腦電位
★ 性侵害犯
★ 情緒和認知交互運作
★ 注意力偏向
★ 情緒史楚普作業
關鍵字(英) ★ The emotion-cognition interaction
★ The emotional Stroop task
★ Attentional bias
★ Sex offender
★ ERP
論文目次 摘要 I
Abstract III
誌謝辭 IV
目錄 VI
圖目錄 VIII
第一章 前言 - 1 -
第二章 文獻回顧 - 4 -
2-1 注意力偏向及情緒性史楚普作業 - 4 -
2-2 注意力偏向的心理背後機制 - 5 -
2-2-1 優先處理模式(Prioritization model) - 6 -
2-2-2 情緒延伸效果(Emotional lingering effect) - 12 -
2-3 情緒性史楚普的大腦相關研究 - 14 -
2-4 性犯罪及煽情刺激的過去認知研究 - 21 -
2-4-1 性侵害行為定義及連續性侵害犯之選擇 - 21 -
2-4-2 性犯罪過去認知研究 - 21 -
2-4-3 以煽情材料為實驗刺激的研究 - 22 -
第三章 實驗一 煽情字詞在情緒性史楚普作業上的應用:叫色反應 - 24 -
3-1 實驗目的 - 24 -
3-2 實驗方法 - 24 -
3-3 實驗結果及討論 - 29 -
第四章 對煽情字詞產生注意力偏向的相關神經機制:ERP情緒性史楚普作業 - 31 -
4-1 實驗2-1:情緒性史楚普作業--按色反應(Manual response) - 31 -
4-1-1 實驗目的 - 31 -
4-1-2 實驗方法 - 32 -
4-1-3 實驗結果及討論 - 35 -
4-2 實驗2-2:從情緒性史楚普作業探討注意力偏向的大腦神經機制 - 37 -
4-2-1 實驗目的 - 37 -
4-2-2 實驗方法 - 38 -
4-2-3 實驗結果 - 41 -
4-2-3-1 行為實驗結果 - 41 -
4-2-3-2 ERP結果分析 - 42 -
4-2-3-3 行為結果及ERP結果之相關 - 44 -
4-2-4 討論 - 47 -
4-3 實驗2-3:ERP情緒性史楚普在連續性侵害犯身上的應用 - 50 -
4-3-1 實驗目的 - 50 -
4-3-2 實驗方法 - 50 -
4-3-3 實驗結果 - 53 -
4-3-3-1 行為實驗結果 - 53 -
4-3-3-2 ERP結果分析 - 54 -
4-3-3-3 行為結果及ERP結果之相關 - 56 -
4-3-4 性侵害犯結果與一般控制組之比較 - 59 -
4-3-4-1行為結果比較 - 60 -
4-3-4-2 ERP結果比較 - 61 -
4-4 討論 - 62 -
第五章 實驗三:圖片情緒性史楚普作業 - 67 -
5-1 實驗目的 - 67 -
5-2 實驗3-1:彩色圖片情緒性史楚普作業 - 68 -
5-2-1 實驗方法 - 68 -
5-2-2 實驗結果及討論 - 71 -
5-3 實驗3-2:灰階圖片情緒性史楚普作業:An ERP study - 73 -
5-3-1 實驗方法 - 74 -
5-3-2 實驗結果 - 76 -
5-3-2-1 行為實驗分析 - 76 -
5-3-2-2 ERP實驗結果分析 - 77 -
5-3-2-3 行為結果及ERP結果之相關 - 88 -
5-4 討論 - 89 -
第六章 綜合討論 - 93 -
參考文獻 - 97 -
附錄 - 103 -
附錄一 本研究使用之字詞刺激材料的詞頻及受試者評量值 - 103 -
附錄二 實驗三所採用的IAPS圖片編號 - 104 -
附錄三 實驗二兩組受試者在ERP成分上的topographic mapping - 105 -
附錄四 實驗3-2兩組受試者在ERP成分上的topographic mapping - 107 -
參考文獻 林明傑,董子毅,陳珍亮和蔡昇運,「台灣性罪犯靜態再犯危險評估量表使用手冊」,國立中正大學,2005年版。
吳吉裕,「連續性侵害加害者之心理描繪因素探索應用研究--從犯罪模式、手法、簽名論之」,國立中正大學,碩士論文,民國92年。
Banich, M. T., Milham, M. P., Atchley, R., Cohen, N. J., Webb, A., Wszalek, T., Kramer, A. F., Liang, Z.-P., Wright, A., Shenker,J., & Magin, R. (2000). “fMRI studies of Stroop tasks reveal unique roles of anterior and posterior brain systems in attentional selection.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 988-1000.
Bernat, E., Bunce, S., Shevrin, H. (2001). “Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing.” International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 11–34.
Bremner, J.D., Vermetten, E., Vythilingam, M., Afzal, N., Schmahl, C., Elzinga, B., Charney, D.S. (2003). “Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder.” Biological psychiatry, 55, 612-620.
Brown, M. & Besner, D. (2001). “On a variant of Stroop's paradigm: Which cognitions press your buttons?” Memory & Cognition, 29, 903-904.
Carretié, L., Martin-Loeches, M., Hinojosa, J.A., Mercado, F. (2001). “Emotion and attention interaction studied through event-related potentials.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 1109–1128.
Carretié, L., Hinojosa, J. A., Martín-Loeches, M., Mercado, F., & Tapia, M. (2004). “Automatic attention to emotional stimuli: Neural correlates.” Human Brain Mapping, 22, 290-299.
Compton, R.J., Banich, M.T., Mohanty, A., Milham, M.P., Herrington, J., Miller, G.A., Scalf, P.E., Webb, A., & Heller, W. (2003). “Paying attention to emotion: an fMRI investigation of cognitive and emotional stroop tasks.” Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience, 3, 81-96.
Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R.J., & Dutton, K. (2001). “Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in sub-clinical anxiety?” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681-700.
Gotlib, I. H. & Cane, D. B.(1987). “Construct accessibility and clinical depression: A longitudinal investigation.” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 199-204.
Hajcak, G., & Nieuwenhuis, S. T. (2006). “Reappraisal modulates the electrocortical response to negative pictures.” Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 6, 291–297.
Hamann, S., Herman, R.A., Nolan, C.L., & Wallen, K. (2004). “Men and women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli.” Nature Neuroscience, 7, 411-416.
Hester, R., Dixon, V., & Garavan, H. (2006). “A consistent attentional bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task.” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 81, 251-257.
Hiatt, K. D., Schmitt, W. A., & Newman, J. P. (2004). “Stroop tasks reveal abnormal selective attention in psychopathic offenders.” Neuropsychology, 18, 50–59.
Johnson, Jr. R. (1993). “On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential.” Psychophysiology, 30, 90–97.
Kolassa, IT., Musial, F., M, A., Trippe, R. H., Miltner, W. HR. (2005). “Electrophysiological correlates of threat processing in spider phobics.” Psychophysiology, 42, 520–530
Larsen, R.J., Mercer, K.A., Balota, D.A. (2006). “Lexical characteristics of words used in emotional Stroop experiments.” Emotion , 6, 62–72.
Li, W., Zinbarg, R.E., & Paller, K.A. (2007). “Trait anxiety modulates supraliminal and subliminal threat: brain potential evidence for early and late processing influences.” Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience, 7, 25-36.
McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (1995). “Intrusive cognitions: An investigation of the emotional Stroop task.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 1595-1607.
McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). “Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: The role of fast and slow components.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 382-392.
MacLeod, C.M. (1991). “Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review.” Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163-203.
Mathews, A., Mogg, K., Kentish J., & Eysenck, M. (1995). “Effect of psychological treatment on cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder.” Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 293-303.
Newman, J. P., Schmitt, W. A., & Voss, W. (1997). “The impact of motivationally neutral cues on psychopathic individuals: Assessing the generality of the response modulation hypothesis.” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 563-575.
Olofsson, J. K., Nordin, S., Sequeira, H., Polich, J. (2008). “Affective picture processing: an integrative review of ERP findings.” Biological Psychology, 77, 247-65
Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P.J. (1995). “Event-related potentials and language comprehension.” In: Rugg, M.D., Coles, M.G.H. (Eds.), Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-related brain potentials and cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 171–215.
Pérez-Edgar, K.& Fox, N.A. (2003). “Individual differences in children's performance during an emotional Stroop task: A behavioral and electrophysiological study” Brain and Cognition, 52, 33-51.
Porter, S., Fairweather, D., Drugge, J., Hervé, H., Birt, A., Boer, D. P. (2000). “Profiles of Psychopathy in Incarcerated Sexual Offenders.” Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27, 216-233.
Powell, M., & Hemsley, D. R. (1984). “Depression: A breakdown of perceptual defence? ” British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 358-362.
Sagaspe, P., Sanchez-Ortuno, M., Charles, A., Taillard, J., Valtat, C., Bioulac, B., Philip, P. (2006). “Effects of sleep deprivation on Color-Word, Emotional, and Specific Stroop interference and on self-reported anxiety.” Brain and Cognition, 60, 76-87.
Sharma, D. & McKenna, F. P. (2001). “The role of time pressure on the emotional Stroop task.” British Journal of Psychology, 92, 471-481.
Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Hillman, C. H., Hamm, A. O., & Lang, P. J. (2004). “Brain processes in emotional perception: Motivated attention.” Cognition and Emotion, 18, 593–611.
Smith, S. S., Arnett, P. A., & Newman, J. P. (1992). “Neuropsychological differentiation of psychopathic andnonpsychopathic criminal offenders.” Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1233-1245.
Smith, P. & Waterman, M. (2004). “Procession bias for sexual material: The emotional Stroop and sexual offender.” Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 16, 163-171.
Thomas, S. J., Johnstone, S.J., & Gonsalvez, C.J. (2007). “Event-related potentials during an emotional Stroop task.” International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 221-231.
van Hooff, J.C., Dietz, K.C., Sharma, D., & Bowman, H. (2008). “Neural correlates of intrusion of emotion words in a modified Stroop task.” International journal of psychophysiology, 67, 23-34.
Vitale, J. E., Brinkley, C. A., Hiatt, K. D., & Newman, J. P. (2007). “Abnormal selective attention in psychopathic female offenders.” Neuropsychology, 21, 301-312.
Williams, J.M.G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). “The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology.” Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24.
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). "The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research." Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441-517.
指導教授 洪蘭(Daisy L. Hung) 審核日期 2008-7-8
推文 facebook   plurk   twitter   funp   google   live   udn   HD   myshare   reddit   netvibes   friend   youpush   delicious   baidu   
網路書籤 Google bookmarks   del.icio.us   hemidemi   myshare   

若有論文相關問題,請聯絡國立中央大學圖書館推廣服務組 TEL:(03)422-7151轉57407,或E-mail聯絡  - 隱私權政策聲明