博碩士論文 982207001 詳細資訊




以作者查詢圖書館館藏 以作者查詢臺灣博碩士 以作者查詢全國書目 勘誤回報 、線上人數:37 、訪客IP:18.117.70.24
姓名 羅于惠(Yu-Hui Lo)  查詢紙本館藏   畢業系所 認知與神經科學研究所
論文名稱 學齡前階段孩童衝突解決和動作反應抑制能力的發展
(The development of conflict monitoring and motor response inhibition in preschool children)
相關論文
★ 時間及空間對注意力暫失的影響 以及其可能的神經生理機制★ 注意力分配及眼球運動準備歷程對於眼動潛伏時間與眼動軌跡的影響
★ 注意力暫失中的數字表徵: 數字距離對注意力暫失的影響★ 利用跨顱磁刺激探討主動式注意力攫取的神經機制
★ 以數學模型及跨顱磁刺激探討注意力分配及眼球運動準備歷程★ 學齡前兒童之視覺注意力發展及電腦化注意力訓練效果之探討
★ 以跨顱磁刺激探討左側下部頂葉以及左側上部頂葉的功能在中文處理中所扮演的角色★ 性侵害犯的衝動行為表現-情緒狀態如何影響性侵害犯的抑制能力?
★ 學齡前階段孩童眼動抑制能力的發展和特性★ 6歲孩童與成人在數字和具體數量上的自動化處理
★ 期望效果之影響與可能的神經機制★ Attentional reorienting: the dynamic interaction between goal-directed and stimulus-driven attentioinal control
★ 前額葉眼動區在視覺搜尋作業上對不同干擾物特徵與顯示時間扮演的角色★ Roles of the Pre-supplementary Motor Area and Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus in Stimulus Selective Stop-signal task: A Theta Burst Transcranial!Magnetic! Stimulation!Study
★ Investigation of posterior parietal cortex visuospatial control over processing in near and far space using transcranial magnetic stimulation★ Using Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation to Investigate the Roles of the Dorsal Lateral Prefrontal Cortex and the Temporoparietal Junction in Top-Down and Bottom-Up Conflict Resolution
檔案 [Endnote RIS 格式]    [Bibtex 格式]    [相關文章]   [文章引用]   [完整記錄]   [館藏目錄]   [檢視]  [下載]
  1. 本電子論文使用權限為同意立即開放。
  2. 已達開放權限電子全文僅授權使用者為學術研究之目的,進行個人非營利性質之檢索、閱讀、列印。
  3. 請遵守中華民國著作權法之相關規定,切勿任意重製、散佈、改作、轉貼、播送,以免觸法。

摘要(中) 抑制能力在學齡前階段具有顯著的進展。過去的研究發現,前額葉受損的兒童(例如:注意力不足暨過動症兒童)在二種類型的抑制能力,衝突解決和動作反應抑制能力的表現較差。因此,本研究主要探討衝突解決和動作反應抑制能力。許多發展性研究顯示,和抑制能力相關的前額葉區域在學齡前階段持續的發展成熟,但卻鮮少有研究探討學齡前階段孩童衝突解決和動作反應抑制能力發展與大腦神經發展之間的關係。
本研究利用兒童版的旁側抑制作業(Flanker task)和停止信號作業(Stop signal task)探討五、六歲孩童的衝突解決和動作反應抑制能力的行為表現和事件相關腦電波。實驗一探討五、六歲孩童執行兒童版旁側抑制作業的一致性效果和N2效果。結果發現五、六歲孩童均觀察到一致性效果和N2效果,但一致性效果和N2效果卻沒有隨著年齡的增長而有顯著性的進展。實驗二探討五、六歲孩童執行停止信號作業時,針對停止信號所需的反應時間和N2成分。行為結果發現看到停止信號時,五歲孩童相較於六歲孩童需要花費較長的時間停止動作反應。然而,五、六歲孩童執行停止信號作業時,在成功動作抑制和失敗成功抑制情境均沒有觀察到N2成分,卻觀察到晚期正向波形(late positive wave)在失敗動作抑制情境較成功抑制情境的振幅大。晚期正向波形反映的是錯誤反應所誘發的正向錯誤成分(error positivity)。此結果顯示五、六歲孩童執行停止訊號作業時,尚未發展出反映動作抑制歷程的N2成分。另一方面晚期正向波形的出現,顯示五、六歲孩童知道自己按錯鍵,且可能開始對此錯誤反應進行改善。
摘要(英) Inhibitory control has a significant improvement in preschool period. Conflict monitoring and motor response inhibition both are types of inhibitory control, and children with frontal lobe dysfunctions, such as attention-deficit hyperactivity children (ADHD), show deficits in these two types of inhibitory control. Many developmental studies have indicated that the prefrontal lobe cortex, which is a region that plays an important role in the inhibitory control, continues mature in preschool stage. However, few studies have integrated the neural correlates of inhibitory control with the development of conflict monitoring and motor response inhibition in typically developing preschool children.
In this thesis, a child version of the flanker task and the stop signal task were used to investigate the behavioral performance and the corresponding event-related potentials (ERPs) of conflict monitoring and motor response inhibition in 5 and 6 years of age. In Experiment 1, we assessed whether congruency effect and the N2 effect were detectable when preschool children performed the child version of the flanker task. Our results showed that the congruency effect and N2 effect (350 - 500 ms) were observed in 5- and 6-years-old children, but did not improve with age between 5 and 6 years. In Experiment 2, we measured the stop signal reaction time (SSRT) and the N2 effect in the stop signal task with 5 and 6 years old preschoolers. The behavioral results indicated that the response inhibition significantly improved with age in 5 and 6 years old. However, we did not observe significant N2 (250 - 450 ms) difference between the successful inhibition condition and the failed inhibition condition in our preschoolers. Instead, we observed that a late positive wave (500 - 900 ms) was more positive-going in the failed inhibition than in the successful inhibition. Late positive component in preschoolers can be associated with the processing of an error event that has been referred to as “error positivity” or Pe. These results suggest that the processes of the inhibitory control indexed by the N2 component have not yet been well-developed when 5 and 6 years old preschoolers performed the stop signal task. On the other hand, it seems that the preschoolers are in the processing of developing the cognitive capability in error detection and correction, as reflected by the late positive component.
關鍵字(中) ★ 抑制能力發展
★ 學齡前階段孩童
★ 事件相關腦電位
關鍵字(英) ★ inhibitory development
★ preschool children
★ event-related potentials
論文目次 摘要 i
Abstract iii
Table of Contents v
List of Figures vii
List of Tables ix
Chapter 1: Introduction 1
1.1 Definition of inhibitory control 2
1.2 Development of inhibitory control 4
1.2.1 Development of conflict monitoring 7
1.2.1.1 Measurement of conflict monitoring 8
1.2.1.2 Development of conflict monitoring: Behavioral evidence 10
1.2.1.3 Development of conflict monitoring: Neurophysiological evidence 11
1.2.2 Development of motor response inhibition 16
1.2.2.1 Measurement of motor response inhibition 17
1.2.2.2 Development of motor response inhibition: Behavioral evidence 20
1.2.2.3 Development of motor response inhibition: Neurophysiological evidence 21
1.3 Purposes of this thesis 22
Chapter 2: Experiment 1 26
2.1 Method 26
2.2 Results 30
2.3 Discussion 36
Chapter 3: Experiment 2 38
3.1 Method 39
3.2 Results 45
3.3 Discussion 53
Chapter 4: General discussion 55
4.1 Experimental findings 55
4.2 Future direction and limitation 62
4.3 Conclusion 63
References 65
參考文獻 Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist, 13(3), 214-228.
Aron, A. R. (2008). Progress in executive-function research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), 124-129.
Aron, A. R., & Verbruggen, F. (2008). Stop the presses: dissociating a selective from a global mechanism for stopping. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1146-1153.
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94.
Barlow, D. H. (Ed.). (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: the nature and treatment of anxiety and panic. New York: Guilford Press.
Berger, A., Jones, L., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Computed games to study the development of attention in childhood. Behavioral Research Methods and Instrumentation, 32, 290-303.
Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 205-228.
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215-222.
Buss, K. A., Dennis, T. A., Brooker, R. J., & Sippel, L. M. (2011). An ERP study of conflict monitoring in 4-8-year old children: Associations with temperament. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 131-140.
Carver, A. C., Livesey, D. J., & Charles, M. (2001a). Age related changes in inhibitory control as measured by stop signal task performance. International Journal of Neuroscience, 107(1-2), 43-61.
Carver, A. C., Livesey, D. J., & Charles, M. (2001b). Further manipulation of the stop-signal task: Developmental changes in the ability to inhibit responding with longer stop-signal delays. International Journal of Neuroscience, 111(1-2), 39-53.
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(3), 104-110.
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R., Giedd, J., Vauss, Y., Vaituzis, C. K., Hamburger, S., et al. (1997). The role of the anterior cingulate in automatic and controlled processes: A developmental neuroanatomical study. Developmental Psychobiology, 30(1), 61-69.
Chambers, C. D., Bellgrove, M. A., Stokes, M. G., Henderson, T. R., Garavan, H., Robertson, I. H., et al. (2006). Executive "brake failure" following deactivation of human frontal lobe. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 444-455.
Chen, C.-Y., Muggleton, N. G., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Juan, C.-H. (2009). Control of prepotent responses by the superior medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage, 44(2), 537-545.
Chen, C. Y., Muggleton, N. G., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Juan, C. H. (2009). Control of prepotent responses by the superior medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage, 44(2), 537-545.
Chugani, H. T. (1998). A critical period of brain development: Studies of cerebral glucose utilization with PET. Preventive Medicine, 27(2), 184-188.
Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: who's in control? Nature Neuroscience, 3(5), 421-423.
Davies, P. L., Segalowitz, S. J., & Gavin, W. J. (2004). Development of response-monitoring ERPs in 7-to 25-year-olds. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25(3), 355-376.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). The early years - Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387-1388.
Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2007). Inhibition of action, thought, and emotion: A selective neurobiological review. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 99-114.
Dimoska, A., Johnstone, S. J., Barry, R. J., & Clarke, A. R. (2003). Inhibitory motor control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Event-related potentials in the stop-signal paradigm. Biological Psychiatry, 54(12), 1345-1354.
Erikson, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.
Falkenstein, M., Hielscher, H., Dziobek, I., Schwarzenau, P., Hoormann, J., Sundermann, B., et al. (2001). Action monitoring, error detection, and the basal ganglia: an ERP study. Neuroreport, 12(1), 157-161.
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP component .2. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78(6), 447-455.
Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial. Biological Psychology, 51(2-3), 87-107.
Fan, J., Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Wu, Y., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(12), 7406-7411.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., & Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26(2), 471-479.
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 133(1), 101-135.
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error-detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4(6), 385-390.
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action - performance of children 31/2 - 7 years old on a stroop - like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129-153.
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10), 861-863.
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(21), 8174-8179.
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1988). Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 11, 137-156.
Gorfein, D. S., & MacLeod, C. M. (Eds.). (2007). The concept of inhibition in cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Hsu, T. Y., Tzeng, L. Y., Yu, J. X., Kuo, W. J., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J. L., et al. (2011). Modulating inhibitory control with direct current stimulation of the superior medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage. (In Press).
Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., & Hennighausen, E. (2000). N200 in the Eriksen-task: Inhibitory executive processes? Journal of Psychophysiology, 14(4), 218-225.
Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Barry, E., Mulligan, A., Daibhis, A., Daly, M., et al. (2008). Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: evidence from the Attention Network Task (ANT). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(12), 1339-1347.
Jonkman, L. M., Sniedt, F. L. F., & Kemner, C. (2007). Source localization of the Nogo-N2: A developmental study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(5), 1069-1077.
Kaiser, J., Barker, R., Haenschel, C., Baldeweg, T., & Gruzelier, J. H. (1997). Hypnosis and event-related potential correlates of error processing in a stroop-type paradigm: a test of the frontal hypothesis. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 27(3), 215-222.
Kok, A., Ramautar, J. R., De Ruiter, M. B., Band, G. P. H., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2004). ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal task. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 9-20.
Kopp, B., Rist, F., & Mattler, U. (1996). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology, 33(3), 282-294.
Ladouceur, C. D., Dahl, R. E., & Carter, C. S. (2007). Development of action monitoring through adolescence into adulthood: ERP and source localization. Developmental Science, 10(6), 874-891.
Lee, H. W., Li, K.-H., Lo, Y.-H., Sung, W.-S., Lin, Y.-C., Ko, H.-W., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Juan, C.-H. (in preparation). Developmental trajectories of motor response inhibition and conflict resolution in preschool children.
Liotti, M., Pliszka, S. R., Perez, R., Luus, B., Glahn, D., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2007). Electrophysiological correlates of response inhibition in children and adolescents with ADHD: Influence of gender, age, and previous treatment history. Psychophysiology, 44(6), 936-948.
Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91(3), 295-327.
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202.
Muggleton, N. G., Chen, C.-Y., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Juan, C.-H. (2010). Inhibitory control and the frontal eye fields. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2804-2812.
Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blow, J., Band, G. P. H., & Kok, A. (2001). Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology, 38(5), 752-760.
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220-246.
Overtoom, C. C. E., Kenemans, J. L., Verbaten, M. N., Kemmer, C., van der Molen, M. W., van Engeland, H., et al. (2002). Inhibition in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A psychophysiological study of the stop task. Biological Psychiatry, 51(8), 668-676.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Educating the human brain. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Ramautar, J. R., Kok, A., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2004). Effects of stop-signal probability in the stop-signal paradigm: The N2/P3 complex further validated. Brain and Cognition, 56(2), 234-252.
Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Vandermolen, M. W. (1995). A psychophysiological analysis of developmental differences in the ability to resist interference. Child Development, 66(4), 1040-1056.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., et al. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029-1040.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Davis-Stober, C. P. (2004). Development of the time course for processing conflict: an event-related potentials study with 4 year olds and adults. Bmc Neuroscience, 5.
Schachar, R., & Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 26(5), 710-720.
Sowell, E. R., Peterson, B. S., Thompson, P. M., Welcome, S. E., Henkenius, A. L., & Toga, A. W. (2003). Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nature Neuroscience, 6(3), 309-315.
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Jernigan, T. L., & Toga, A. W. (1999). In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10), 859-861.
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(38), 8223-8231.
Thorell, L. B. (2007). Do delay aversion and executive function deficits make distinct contributions to the functional impact of ADHD symptoms? A study of early academic skill deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(11), 1061-1070.
Tillman, C. M., Thorell, L. B., Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin, G. (2008). Motor response inhibition and execution in the stop-signal task: Development and relation to ADHD behaviors. Child Neuropsychology, 14(1), 42-59.
Trommer, B. L., Hoeppner, J. A. B., & Zecker, S. G. (1991). The go-no go test in attention-deficit disorder is sensitive to methylphenidate. Journal of Child Neurology, 6, S128-S131.
van Boxtel, G. J. M., van der Molen, M. W., Jennings, J. R., & Brunia, C. H. M. (2001). A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm. Biological Psychology, 58(3), 229-262.
van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 593-602.
Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 205-213.
Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111(4), 931-959.
指導教授 阮啟弘(Chi-Hung Juan) 審核日期 2011-7-22
推文 facebook   plurk   twitter   funp   google   live   udn   HD   myshare   reddit   netvibes   friend   youpush   delicious   baidu   
網路書籤 Google bookmarks   del.icio.us   hemidemi   myshare   

若有論文相關問題,請聯絡國立中央大學圖書館推廣服務組 TEL:(03)422-7151轉57407,或E-mail聯絡  - 隱私權政策聲明