參考文獻 |
Andres, M., Davare, M., Pesenti, M., Olivier, E., & Seron, X. (2004). Number
magnitude and grip aperture interaction. Neuroreport, 15(18), 2773-2777.
Bachtold, D. B., M., & Brugger, P. (1998). Stimulus-response compatibility in
representational space. Neuropsychologia, 36(8), 731-735.
Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., & Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting Parity and
Magnitude from Arabic Numerals: Developmental Changes in Number Processing
and Mental Representation,. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(4),
286-308.
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does Language Shape Thought?: Mandarin and English
Speakers' Conceptions of Time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1-22.
Brannon, E. M., & Terrace, H. S. (2000). Representation of the numerosities 1-9 by
rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior, 26(1), 31-49.
Caessens, B., Hommel, B., Reynvoet, B., & Van der Goten, K. (2004).
Backward-compatibility effects with irrelevant stimulus-response overlap: The
case of the SNARC effect. Journal of General Psychology, 131(4), 411-425.
Calabria, M. A.,& Rossetti, Y. (2005). Interference between number processing and line bisection: a methodology. Neuropsychologia, 43(5), 779-783.
Cheng SK., Tsai, CC., Lee, JR., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. JL. (2005, April). Comparing Numbers in Arabic Digits, Complex/Simple Chinese Numerals and Dot Arrays: An Event and Dot Arrays: An Event-Related Potential (ERP) Study. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, New York.
Clark, H. (1973). Space, time semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language. New York: Academic Press.
Dehaene, S. (1989). The psychophysics of numerical comparison: a reexamination of
apparently incompatible data. Perceptual Psychophysics, 6, 557-566.
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44(1-2), 1-42.
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S. & Giraux P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and
numerical magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122,
371--396.
Delazer, M., & Butterworth, B. (1997). A dissociation of number meanings. Cognitive
neurophysiology, 14(4), 613?36.
Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of
magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC
effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2, 95-110.
Fias, W. (2001). Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: evidence from the
SNARC effect. Psychological Research, 65(4), 250-259.
Fias, W., Lauwereyns, J., & Lammertyn, J. (2001). Irrelevant digits affect
feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. Cognitive
Brain Research, 12(3), 415-423.
Fischer, M., Warlop, N., Hill, R., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by
number perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91-97.
Fischer, M. H. (2001). Number processing induces spatial performance biases.
Neurology, 57(5), 822-826.
Fischer, M. H., Castel, A.D., Dodd, M.D., Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes
spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555-556.
Gentner, D. (2001). Spatial metaphors in temporal reasoning. In M. Gattis (Ed.),
Spatial schemas in abstract thought (pp. 203-222). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing
architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of
Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), 659 - 673.
Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal
sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 87(3), B87-B95.
Gevers, W., Lammertyn, J., Notebaert, W., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica, In Press, Corrected Proof.
Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261-268.
Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between Number and Space in Parietal Cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(6), 435-448.
Huntley-Fenner, G. & Cannon., E. (2000). Preschoolers' Magnitude Comparisons are
Mediated by a Preverbal Analog Mechanism. Psychological Science, 11(2),
147-152.
Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers:
evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 32(4), 662-673.
Keus, I. M., Jenks, K. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Psychophysiological evidence that
the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response selection stage. Cognitive
Brain Research, 24(1), 48-56.
Kornblum, S. H asbroucq., T.; Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253-270.
Lavidor, M., Brinksman, V., & Gobel, S. M. (2004). Hemispheric asymmetry and the
mental number line: comparison of double-digit numbers. Neuropsychologia,
42(14), 1927-1933.
Mapelli, D., Rusconi, E., & Umilta, C. (2003). The SNARC effect: an instance of the
Simon effect? Cognition, 88(3), B1-B10.
Nieder, A., & Miller, E. K. (2004). Analog Numerical Representations in Rhesus
Monkeys: Evidence for Parallel Processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
16(5), 889-901.
Nieder, A., & Miller, E. K. (2004). A parieto-frontal network for visual numerical
information in the monkey. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 101(19), 7457-7462.
Nuerk, H., Wood, G, & Willmes, K. (2005). The universal SNARC effect: the
association between number magnitude and space is amodal. Experimental
Psychology, 52(3), 187-194.
Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and Approximate
Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group. Science, 306(5695), 499-503.
Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Riviere, D., & LeBihan, D. (2001). Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task. NeuroImage, 14(5), 1013-1026.
Pinel, P., Piazza, M., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron, 41(6), 983-993.
Ratinckx, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2002). Interhemispheric stroop-like interference in
number comparison: evidence for strong interhemispheric integration of semantic
number information. Neuropsychology, 16(2), 217-229.
Rossetti, Y., Jacquin-Courtois, S., Rode, G., Ota, H., Michel, C., & Boisson, D. (2004). Does Action Make the Link Between Number and Space Representation?.
Visuo-Manual Adaptation Improves Number Bisection in Unilateral Neglect.
Psychological Science, 15(6), 426-430.
Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial
representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition, 199(2), 113-129.
Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line:
comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perceptual
Psychophysics, 66(4), 651-664.
Simon, O., Mangin, JF., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation, and language-related areas in the human parietal lobe. Neuron, 33(3), 475-487.
Temple, E., & Posner, M. I. (1998). Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in
5-year-old children and adults. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 95(13), 7836-7841.
Turconi, E., & Seron, X. (2002). Dissociation between order and quantity meanings in a patient with Gerstmann syndrome. Cortex, 38, 911-914.
Turconi, E., Campbell, J. I. D., & Seron, X. (2006). Numerical order and quantity
processing in number comparison. Cognition, 98(3), 273-285.
Turconi, E., Jemel, B., Rossion, B., & Seron, X. (2004). Electrophysiological
evidence for differential processing of numerical quantity and order in humans.
Cognitive Brain Research, 21(1), 22-38.
Vuilleumier, P., Ortigue, S., Brugger, P. (2004). The number space and neglect. Cortex, 40(2), 399-410.
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and
quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483-488.
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. 358(6389), 749-750.
Xu, F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month-old
infants. Cognition, 74(1), B1-B11.
Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umilta, C. (2002). Brain damage Neglect disrupts the mental
number line. Nature, 417(6885), 138-139. |