博碩士論文 101295001 詳細資訊




以作者查詢圖書館館藏 以作者查詢臺灣博碩士 以作者查詢全國書目 勘誤回報 、線上人數:70 、訪客IP:18.227.0.255
姓名 謝宜儒(I-Ju Hsieh)  查詢紙本館藏   畢業系所 認知與神經科學研究所
論文名稱 道德決策判斷:以道德基礎理論探討道德基本原則如何形塑個體的政治意識形態、社會議題之態度與司法判決
(Moral decision making: An examination of moral foundations theory in the formation of political ideology, attitudes toward social issues, and judicial judgments)
相關論文
★ Cultural Orientation and Acculturation: An Exploration of Measures of Cultural Orientation and Interplay of Factors Involved in Cross-Cultural Adjustment of Swazi Sojourners in Taiwan★ 基於獨立EEG成分的運動執行、運動想像和運動觀察的源識別及比較乾/濕電極的獨立成分差異
★ 探討P300事件關聯電位特徵用於分析隱藏訊息測驗之腦波資料的效果★ How the Brain Deals with the Asymmetry between Chinese Subject and Object Relative Clauses: Support for a Dynamic Processing Theory
★ Increased Cognitive Load with Spatial Cueing Task Augments the P300 Amplitude Differences in EEG-based CIT Experiments★ 以毛筆筆刷、手掌觸摸、和指尖輕點的觸覺刺激探討情動觸摸所引 發的theta 頻段腦波強度變化
★ 運用VGG網絡對靜息態功能性磁振造影成分圖進行區分
檔案 [Endnote RIS 格式]    [Bibtex 格式]    [相關文章]   [文章引用]   [完整記錄]   [館藏目錄]   [檢視]  [下載]
  1. 本電子論文使用權限為同意立即開放。
  2. 已達開放權限電子全文僅授權使用者為學術研究之目的,進行個人非營利性質之檢索、閱讀、列印。
  3. 請遵守中華民國著作權法之相關規定,切勿任意重製、散佈、改作、轉貼、播送,以免觸法。

摘要(中) 道德是個體用來判斷好壞與對錯的基礎。它不僅會影響在假設情境中的判斷與抉擇,也會影響個體的政治意識形態以及對於各種備受爭議之議題的態度,更甚者,當個體的態度轉變成為實際的懲罰行為,道德依然是一個決定性的因素。研究一探討,面臨犧牲少數人拯救多數人的難題時,不同的決策方式和肢體接觸對於個體的道德決策判斷是否產生影響。共345名參與者完成道德兩難困境。在道德判斷中,個體以非自我中心觀點的方式對事件進行評價時,有無肢體接觸對參與者來說並不影響最後判斷的結果。在道德抉擇中,個體以自我中心的觀點進行評估,行動中是否包含肢體接觸就是個重要的關鍵。研究二探討,在台灣社會中道德觀對於政治意識形態和社會議題的態度。共835位參與者完成道德基本原則問卷、意識形態一致性量表與對於社會議題的立場。自由派的參與者較注重關懷和公平,而保守派的參與者則注重聖潔;注重權威與聖潔的參與者也分別支持實施死刑與反對安樂死。研究三探討個體道德觀對於其司法判斷結果的影響,102位參與者完成道德基本原則問卷以及司法判斷題目。當針對假設的犯罪行為進行量刑,一般民眾比專業法律人士更容易受到年齡與教育程度的影響,也比較容易在參照標準浮動的時候改變量刑,專業法律人士則否。綜合以上,道德觀影響範圍深遠,在個體的態度和行為上也有不同的影響,並具有一定程度上的文化普同性。最後,透過專業訓練,專業法律人士進行司法審判的量刑標準時,可能得以排除與案件無關的干擾因素,進而不受影響。
摘要(英) Morality is often described as a compass, that provides directions for good and bad, right and wrong, which may include both universal and subjective aspects. People rely on morality to evaluate situations, make judgments or decisions that lead to behaviors. Study 1 examined the relationship between physical contact and decision type in predicting “harm to save” behavior. In the choice condition only, participants reported significantly less utilitarian responses to the dilemmas that required having physical contact with the person to be harmed than dilemmas that did not require physical contact. This difference was not found in the judgment condition. Study 2 examined the relationships among moral foundations, political ideology, and controversial social issues in an Asian culture. Results indicated that binding foundations (i.e. Ingroup, Authority, and Purity) were associated with conservative tendencies, and individualizing foundations (i.e. Harm and Fairness) were associated with liberal tendencies. Also, participants who scored higher on Authority showed higher approval of death penalty, and those scored higher on Purity showed lower approval of euthanasia. Study 3 examined the associations among moral foundations, judicial judgments, and judgment bias. Results showed that age and education level predicted judgment in the community group only was. This group also registered significant changes to their decisions when the reference point changed, while the legal professional group did not register such change Results from these studies showed the wide range of effects that morality, from conceptual to practical aspects, on decision making, stands on controversial issues, as well as behaviors.
關鍵字(中) ★ 道德判斷
★ 道德抉擇
★ 道德基礎理論
★ 司法判斷
★ 政治意識形態
關鍵字(英) ★ moral judgment
★ moral choice
★ moral foundations theory
★ juducial judgment
★ political ideology
論文目次 目錄
中文摘要 ii
Abstract iii
目錄 iv
圖目錄 ix
第一章 緒論 1
第一節 研究背景 1
第二節 研究動機與目的 2
第三節 章節安排 3
第二章 文獻探討 4
第一節 道德決策判斷中的心智歷程 4
壹、道德推理形成道德決策判斷 4
貳、道德直覺先於道德推理 7
參、認知與情感歷程相互競爭 10
第二節 道德決策判斷中的情緒喚起 14
壹、義務論與效益論 14
貳、道德決策判斷中的心理距離 17
參、判斷與抉擇 18
第三節 道德決策判斷中的普世性與相對性 20
壹、跨文化的證據 20
貳、共通的道德原則模組 25
參、文化內群體的差異 29
第四節 道德基本原則與司法判斷 32
壹、法庭中的認知偏誤 32
貳、偏誤的外在與內在因素 34
第五節 研究目的與假設 38
壹、肢體接觸與決策方式對於道德判斷的交互作用影響 38
參、道德基本原則對於司法判斷的影響 40
第三章 研究一 41
第一節 研究樣本 41
第二節 研究工具 41
第三節 施測程序 42
第四節 分析方法 42
第五節 分析結果與討論 43
壹、描述性統計 43
貳、控制變項分析 43
參、肢體接觸與決策方式主要效果分析 43
肆、交互作用結果分析 44
伍、族群差異 45
陸、討論與限制 46
(一)決策方式對於道德決策判斷的影響 46
(二)肢體接觸對於道德決策判斷的影響 47
(三)限制 47
柒、小結 49
第四章 研究二 50
第一節 研究樣本 50
第二節 研究工具 50
壹、道德基本原則 50
貳、政治意識形態 51
參、社會議題 51
第三節 施測程序 52
第四節 分析方法 52
第五節 分析結果與討論 52
壹、描述性統計與相關性分析 52
貳、道德基本原則與政治意識形態關聯性 54
參、道德基本原則與社會議題關聯性 57
肆、討論與限制 57
(一)道德基本原則與政治意識形態 58
(二)道德基本原則與社會議題 60
伍、小結 61
第五章 研究三 62
第一節 研究樣本 62
第二節 研究工具 62
壹、道德基本原則 62
貳、司法判斷作業 63
參、同理心 63
肆、政治意識形態 64
第三節 施測程序 64
第四節 分析方法 64
第五節 分析結果與討論 65
壹、描述性統計 65
貳、道德基本原則、司法判斷與同理心之族群比較 68
(一)道德基本原則 68
(二)同理心 69
(三)司法判斷 69
(四)政治意識形態 70
參、以道德基本原則預測司法判斷作業 70
(一)學生組 70
(二)社區民眾組 71
(三)專業法律人士組 71
肆、個體化與合併道德基本原則預測司法判斷作業 75
(一)學生組 75
(二)社區民眾組 75
(三)專業法律人士組 76
伍、區組間比較 80
陸、區分專業法律與非專業法律人士 81
(一)道德基本原則問卷 81
(二)道德基本原則短句 82
(三)區組間差異 83
柒、討論與限制 84
(一)組別在道德基本原則、同理心與司法判斷差異 84
(二)以道德基本原則預測司法判斷作業 87
(三)個體化與合併道德基本原則預測司法判斷作業 92
(五)研究限制 94
第六章 綜合討論 97
參考文獻 100
附錄 124
附錄一 道德兩難困境 124
附錄二 道德基本原則問卷 127
附錄三 意識形態一致性量表 129
附錄四 社會議題 130
附錄五 道德基本原則短句 131
參考文獻 參考文獻
Abarbanell, L., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). Mayan morality: An exploration of permissible harms. Cognition, 115, 207-224.
Ahlenius, H., & Tännsjö, T. (2012). Chinese and Westerners Respond Differently to the Trolley Dilemmas Journal of Cognition and Culture, 12(3/4), 195-201.
AlSheddi, M., Russell, S., & Hegarty, P. (2019). How does culture shape our moral identity? Moral foundations in Saudi Arabia and Britain. European Journal of Social Psychology doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2619
An, S., & Trafimow, D. (2014). Affect and Morality : A Cross-Cultural Examination of Moral Attribution. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(3), 417-430. doi: 10.1177/0022022113511298
Arutyunova, K. R., Alexandrov, Y. I., Znakov, V. V., & Hauser, M. D. (2013). Moral Judgments in Russian Culture: Universality and Cultural Specificity. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 13(3-4), 255 - 285. doi: 10.1163/15685373-12342094
Banerjee, K., Huebner, B., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). Intuitive Moral Judgments are Robust across Variation in Gender, Education, Politics and Religion: A Large-Scale Web-Based Study. . Journal of Cognition & Culture, 10(3/4), 253-281. doi: 10.1163/156853710X531186
Bartling, B., & Schmidt, K. (2013). Reference Points, Social Norms, and Fairness inContract Renegotiations. Kiel und Hamburg.
Batson, C. D., & Thompson, E. R. (2001). Why Don′t Moral People Act Morally? Motivational Considerations Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(2), 54 - 57. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00114
Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Evil : Inside Human Cruelty and Violence. New York: W.H. Freeman.
Benforado, A. (2016). Unfair: The New Science of Criminal Injustice: Broadway Books.
Bentham, J. (1789). An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation.
Billings, R. S., & Scherer, L. L. (1988). The effects of response mode and importance on decision-making strategies: Judgment versus choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 41(1), 1-19. doi: dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(88)90043-X
Blum, L. A. (1988). Gilligan and Kohlberg: Implications for Moral Theory. Ethics, 98(3), 472-491. doi: 10.1086/292966
Boven, L. V., Kane, J., McGraw, A. P., & Dale, J. (2010). Feeling Close: Emotional Intensity Reduces Perceived Psychological Distance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98 (6), 872–885. doi: 10.1037/a0019262
Boyle, B. A., Dahlstrom, R. F., & Kellaris, J. J. (1998). Points of Reference and Individual Differences As Sources of Bias in Ethical Judgments. Journal of Business Ethics, 17(5), 517–525. doi: 10.1023/A:1017960518974
Chen, M., Li, Y., & Gao, W. (2016). Priming modernity and work experiences strengthens the association between fairness/harm concerns and anger in China. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 40-48. doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12124
Chiou, J.-S. (2001). Horizontal and Vertical Individualism and Collectivism Among College Students in the United States, Taiwan, and Argentina The Journal of Social Psychology, 141(5), 667-678. doi: 10.1080/00224540109600580
Cho, K., Barnes, C. M., & Guanara, C. L. (2017). Sleepy Punishers Are Harsh Punishers: Daylight Saving Time and Legal Sentences. Psychological Science, 28(2), 242 - 247. doi: 10.1177/0956797616678437
Cho, Y.-H., & Yoon, J. (2001). The Origin and Function of Dynamic Collectivism: An Analysis of Korean Corporate Culture. Asia Pacific Business Review,, 7(4), 70-88. doi: 10.1080/713999116
Christensen, J. F., Flexas, A., Miguel, P. d., Cela-Conde, C. J., & Munar, E. (2014). Roman Catholic beliefs produce characteristic neural responses to moral dilemmas. SCAN, 9, 240-249.
Cima, M., Tonnaer, F., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). Psychopaths know right from wrong but don’t care Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(1), 59 - 67. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp051
Clifford, S., Iyengar, V., Cabeza, R., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2015). Moral foundations vignettes: a standardized stimulus database of scenarios based on moral foundations theory. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 1178–1198. doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0551-2
Costa, A., Foucart, A., Hayakawa, S., Aparici, M., Apesteguia, J., Heafner, J., & Keysar, B. (2014). Your Morals Depend on Language. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e94842. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094842
Cushman, F., Young, L., & Hauser, M. D. (2006). The Role of Conscious Reasoning and Intuition in Moral Judgment: Testing Three Principles of Harm. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1082 - 1089. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01834.x
Danziger, S., Levav, J., & Avnaim-Pesso, L. (2011). Extraneous factors in judicial decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(17). doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018033108
Davis, J. I., Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2011). Psychological Distance and Emotional Experience:What You See Is What You Get. Emotion, 11(2), 438–444. doi: 0.1037/a0021783
Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. SAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10.
Dawson, S. L., & Tyson, G. A. (2012). Will Morality or Political Ideology Determine Attitudes to Climate Change. Australian Community Psychologist, 24(2), 8-25.
Deak, C., & Saroglou, V. (2015). Opposing Abortion, Gay Adoption, Euthanasia, and Suicide Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 37, 267-294. doi: 10.1163/15736121-12341309
Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., & Kinzler, K. D. (2012). The Contribution of Emotion and Cognition to Moral Sensitivity: A Neurodevelopmental Study. Cerebral Cortex January, 22, 209-220. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr111
Ellemers, N., Toorn, J. v. d., Paunov, Y., & Leeuwen, T. v. (2019). The Psychology of Morality: A Review and Analysis of Empirical Studies Published From 1940 Through 2017. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1088868318811759. doi: 10.1177/1088868318811759
Englich, B., Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (2006). Playing Dice With Criminal Sentences: The Influence of Irrelevant Anchors on Experts’ Judicial Decision Making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(2), 188-200. doi: 10.1177/0146167205282152
Faigman, D. L., Kang, J., Bennett, M. W., Carbado, D. W., Casey, P., Dasgupta, N., . . . Mnookin, J. (2012). Implicit Bias in the Courtroom. UCLA Law Review, 59, 1124-1187.
Fischer, D. B. R. (2013). How and When Do Personal Values Guide Our Attitudes and Sociality? Explaining Cross-Cultural Variability in Attitude-Value Linkages. Psychological Bullccin, 139(5), 1113-1147. doi: 10.1037/a003l.147
Gao, W., Chen, M., & Li, Y. (2016). Descriptive norms influence reactions to anger-related moral events. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 31-39. doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12121
Gao, Y., & Tang, S. (2013). Psychopathic personality and utilitarian moral judgment in college students. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41(5), 342 - 349. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2013.06.012
Gilligan, C. (1977). In a different voice: Women’s conceptions of the self and of morality. Harvard Educational Review, 47(4), 481-517. doi: 10.17763/haer.47.4.g6167429416hg5l0
Gleichgerrcht, E., & Young, L. (2013). Low Levels of Empathic Concern Predict Utilitarian MoralJudgment. PLoS ONE, 8(4). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060418
Glynn, A. N., & Sen, M. (2015). Identifying Judicial Empathy: Does Having Daughters Cause Judges to Rule for Women′s Issues? : IDENTIFYING JUDICIAL EMPATHY American Journal of Political Science, 59(1), 37-54. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12118
Gold, N., Colman, A. M., & Pulford, B. D. (2014). Cultural differences in responses to real-life and hypothetical trolley problems. Judgment And Decision Making, 9(1), 65-76. doi: 10.1177/0272431698018002004
Grühn, D., Rebucal, K., Diehl, M., Lumley, M., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (2008). Empathy Across the Adult Lifespan: Longitudinal and Experience Sampling Findings. Emotion, 8(6), 753 - 765. doi: 10.1037/a0014123
Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral Foundations Theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 55-130. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029 - 1046. doi: 10.1037/a0015141
Graham, J., Meindl, P., Beall, E., Johnson, K. M., & Zhang, L. (2016). Cultural differences in moral judgment and behavior, ccross and within societies. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8(125), 125-130. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.007
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., & Haidt, J. (2012). The Moral Stereotypes of Liberals and Conservatives: Exaggeration of Differences across the Political Spectrum. PLoS ONE, 7(12). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050092
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the Moral Domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366-385. doi: 10.1037/a0021847
Greene, J. D. (2007). The secret joke of Kant ’ s soul. In W. S.-. Armstrong (Ed.), Moral psychology: Vol. 2. The cognitive science of morality (pp. 35 - 79). Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.
Greene, J. D., Cushman, F. A., Stewart, L. E., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2009). Pushing moral buttons: The interaction between personal force and intention in moral judgment. Cognition, 111(3), 364 - 371. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.001
Greene, J. D., Morelli, S. A., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition, 107(3), 1144 - 1154. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.004
Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment. SCIENCE, 293, 2105-2108. doi: 10.1126/science.1062872
Gump, L. S., Baker, R. C., & Roll, S. (2000). Cultural and Gender Differences in Moral Judgment: A Study of Mexican Americans and Anglo-Americans Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 22(1), 78 - 93. doi: 10.1177/0739986300221004
Guthrie, C., Rachlinski, J. J., & Wistrich, A. J. (2001). Inside the Judicial Mind. Cornell Law Faculty Publications, 86, 777-829.
Guthrie, C., Rachlinski, J. J., & Wistrich, A. J. (2007). Blinking on the Bench: How Judges Decide Cases. Cornell Law Faculty Publications, 93(1).
Haidt, J. (2001). The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814-834. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X. 108.4.814
Haidt, J. (2013). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion: Vintage.
Haidt, J., & Baron, J. (1996). Social roles and the moral judgement of acts and omissions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26(2), 201-218.
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When Morality Opposes Justice: Conservatives Have Moral Intuitions that Liberals may not Recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98 - 116. doi: 10.1007/s11211-007-0034-z
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: how innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 55 - 66. doi: 10.1162/0011526042365555
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How five sets of innate intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. The innate mind, 3, 367-391. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195332834.003.0019
Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S. (2010). Morality. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 797-832). NJ: Hobeken.
Haidt, J., Roller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, Culture, and Morality, or Is It Wrong to Eat your Dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613-628. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.613
Hamamura, T., Heine, S. J., & Paulhus, D. L. (2008). Cultural differences in response styles: The roleof dialectical thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 932–942. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.034
Hannikainen, I. R., Machery, E., & Cushman, F. A. (2018). Is utilitarian sacrifice becoming more morally permissible? Cognition, 170, 95-101. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.013
Hauser, M., Cushman, F., Young, L., Jin, R. K.-X., & Mikhail, J. (2007). A Dissociation Between Moral Judgments and Justifications Mind & Language, 22(1), 1-21.
Hauser, M. D., Tonnaer, F., & Cima, M. (2009). When Moral Intuitions Are Immune to the Law: A Case Study of Euthanasia and the Act-Omission Distinction in The Netherlands. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 9, 149 - 169. doi: 10.1163/156770909X12489459066147
Henrich, J. (2015). Culture and social behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 84-89. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.02.001
Hofmann, W., Wisneski, D. C., Brandt, M. J., & Skitka, L. J. (2014). Morality in everyday life. SCIENCE, 345, 1340 - 1343. doi: 10.1126/science.1251560
Holyoak, K. J., & Mah, W. A. (1982). Cognitive reference points in judgments of symbolic magnitude. Cognitive Psychology, 14(3), 328-352. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90013-5
Irwin, J. F., & Real, D. L. (2016). Unconscious Infl ue nces on Judicial Decision-Making: Th e Illusion of Objectivity. McGeorge Law Review, 42(1).
Jackson, M., Pippin, S., & Wong, J. (2014). Court Rulings in Estate Tax Cases: Is Gender a Factor? . The ATA Journal of Legal Tax Research, 12(2), 74-84. doi: 10.2308/jltr-50914
Jahoda, G., & Stacey, B. (1970). Susceptibility to geometrical illusions according to culture and professional training. Perception & Psychophysics, 7(3), 179–184. doi: 10.3758/BF03208653
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political Conservatism as Motivated Social Cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339 - 375. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its Resurgence in Social, Personality, and Political Psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(2), 126 - 136. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00070.x
Kahan, D. M., Hoffman, D., Evans, D., Devins, N., Lucci, E., & Cheng, K. (2016). “Ideology” or “Situation Sense”? An Experimental Investigation of Motivated Reasoning and Professional Judgment. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 164, 349-439.
Kahane, G., & Shackel, N. (2010). Methodological Issues in the Neuroscience of Moral Judgement Mind & Language, 25(5), 561-582. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01401.x
Kim, K. R., Kang, J.-S., & Yun, S. (2012). Moral Intuitions and Political Orientation: Similarities and Differences between South Korea and the United States Psychological Reports, 111(1), 173 - 185. doi: 10.2466/17.09.21.PR0.111.4.173-185
Kim, S. H. (2002). Anti‐Americanism in Korea. Washington Quarterly, 26(1), 109-122. doi: 10.1162/016366003761036525
Koenigs, M., Kruepke, M., Zeier, J., & Newman, J. P. (2012). Utilitarian moral judgment in psychopathy. SCAN, 7, 708-714. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr048
Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M. D., & Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446, 908-911. doi: 10.1038/nature05631
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive development approach to socialization. In Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory (pp. 347-480). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
Koleva, S., Selterman, D., Iyer, R., Ditto, P., & Graham, J. (2014). The Moral Compass of Insecurity : Anxious and Avoidant Attachment Predict Moral Judgment Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(2), 185-194. doi: 10.1177/1948550613490965
Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., Ditto, P. H., & Haidt, J. (2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially Purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 184 - 194. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.006
Krettenauer, T., Murua, L. A., & Jia, F. (2016). Age-Related Differences in Moral Identity Across Adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 52(6), 972–984. doi: 10.1037/dev0000127
Kurzban, R., DeSciol, P., & Fein, D. (2012). Hamilton vs. Kant: pitting adaptations for altruism against adaptations for moral judgment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 323-333. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.11.002
Kwan, L. Y.-Y. (2016). Anger and perception of unfairness and harm: Cultural differences in normative processes that justify sanction assignment. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 6-15. doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12119
Lawrence, L. A. (2011). Work Engagement, Moral Distress, Education Level, and Critical Reflective Practice in Intensive Care Nurses. Nursing forum, 46(4), 256-268. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6198.2011.00237.x
Lee, S. W. S., Tang, H., Wan, J., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2015). A cultural look at moral purity: wiping the face clean. Frontiers in Psychology 6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00577
Li, F., Chao, M. C.-h., Chen, N. Y.-f., & Zhang, S. (2017). Moral judgment in a business setting: The role of managers’ moral foundation, ideology, and level of moral development Asia Pacific Journal of Management. doi: 10.1007/s10490-017-9529-9
Milesi, P. (2016). Moral foundations and political attitudes: The moderating role of political sophistication. International Journal of Psychology, 51(4), 252–260. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12158
Moore, A. B., Lee, N. Y. L., Clark, B. A. M., & Conway, A. R. A. (2011). In defense of the personal/impersonal distinction in moral psychology research: Cross-cultural validation of the dual process model of moral judgment. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(3), 186-195.
Mullins-Nelson, J. L., Salekin, R. T., Leistico, A.-M. R., & Cabeza, R. (2012). Psychopathy, Empathy, and Perspective -Taking Ability in a Community Sample: Implications for the Successful Psychopathy Concept. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 5(2), 133-149. doi: 10.1080/14999013.2006.10471238
Nilsson, A., & Erlandsson, A. (2015). The Moral Foundations taxonomy: Structural validity and relation to political ideology in Sweden. Personality and Individual Differences, 76(28-32). doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.049
O′Neill, P., & Petrinovich, L. (1998). A Preliminary Cross-Cultural Study of Moral Intuitions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 349-367.
Oeberst, A., & Goeckenjan, I. (2016). When Being Wise After the Event Results in Injustice: Evidence for Hindsight Bias in Judges’ Negligence Assessments. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 22(3), 271 - 279. doi: 10.1037/law0000091
Paxton, J. M., & Greene, J. D. (2010). Moral Reasoning: Hints and Allegations. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 511–527. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01096.x
Phillips, L. H., MacLean, R. D. J., & Allen, R. (2002). Age and the Understanding of Emotions: Neuropsychological and Sociocognitive Perspectives. Journal of Gerontology, 57B(6), 526-530. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.6.P526
Pletti, C., Lotto, L., Buodo, G., & Sarlo, M. (2017). It′s immoral, but I′d do it! Psychopathy traits affect decision-making in sacrificial dilemmas and in everyday moral situations British Journal of Psychology, 108, 1351–1368. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12205
Pratt, M. W., Golding, G., & Hunter, W. J. (1983). Aging as ripening: Character and consistency of moral judgment in young, mature, and older adults. Human Development, 26(5), 277-288. doi: 10.1159/000272889
Prince, C. (2010). Moral Foundation Theory and the Law. Seattle University Law Review, 33(4), 1293-1317.
Raaijmakers, Q. A. W., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Hoof, A. V. (2005). Delinquency and moral reasoning in adolescence and young adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(3), 247-258. doi: 10.1080/01650250544000035
Rachlinski, J. J., Johnson, S. L., Wistrich, A. J., & Guthrie, C. (2009). Does Unconscious Bias Affect Trial Judges? . Notre Dame L. Review, 84, 1195-1246. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.999490
Rai, T. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (2013). Exposure to moral relativism compromises moral behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.06.008
Redding, R. E., & Reppucci, N. D. (1999). Effects of Lawyers′ Socio-political Attitudes on Their Judgments of Social Science in Legal Decision Making. Law and Human Behavior, 23(1), 31-54. doi: 10.1023/a:1022322706533
Rest, J. R., Narvaez, D., Thoma, S. J., & Bebeau, M. J. (1999). DIT2: Devising and Testing a Revised Instrument of Moral Judgment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(4), 644-659. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.644
Rest, J. R., & Thoma, S. J. (1985). Relation of Moral Judgment Development to Formal Education. Developmental Psychology, 21(4), 709-714. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.21.4.709
Romand, P., Pantaléon, N., & Cabagno, G. (2009). Age Differences in Individuals’ Cognitive and Behavioral Moral Functioning Responses in Male Soccer Teams. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21(1), 49-63. doi: 10.1080/10413200802383055
Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 681 - 690. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.57.4.681
Rosik, C. H., Dinges, L. J., & Saavedra, N. (2013). Moral Intuitions and Attitudes toward Gay Men: Can Moral Psychology Add to Our Understanding of Homonegativity? . Journal of Psychology and Theology, 41(4), 315 - 326. doi: 10.1177/009164711304100404
Rostan, S. M. (1994). Problem finding, problem solving, and cognitive controls: An empirical investigation of critically acclaimed productivity. Creativity Research Journal 7(2), 97-110. doi: 10.1080/10400419409534517
Rottman, J., Kelemen, D., & Young, L. (2014). Tainting the soul: Purity concerns predict moral judgments of suicide. Cognition, 130(2), 217–226. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.007
Sağel, E. (2015). Age differences in moral foundations across adolescene and adulthood. (Master), MIDDLE EAST TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY
Sapp, G. L., & Jones, L. (1986). Religious Orientation and Moral Judgment Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 25(2), 208-214. doi: 10.2307/1385477
Schieman, S., & Gundy, K. V. (2000). The Personal and Social Links between Age and Self-Reported Empathy. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63(2), 152-174. doi: 10.2307/2695889
Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008). With a Clean Conscience : Cleanliness Reduces the Severity of Moral Judgments. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1219 - 1222. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02227.x
Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1982). Does the Concept of the Person Vary Cross-Culturally? . In A. J. Marsella & G. M. White (Eds.), Cultural Conceptions of Mental Health and Therapy (pp. 97-137).
Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and moral development. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Schweder & G. Herdt (Eds.), The Emergence of Morality in Young Children. Chicago: Cambridge University Press
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The ”big three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the ”big three” explanations of suffering. In A. M. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 119-169). Florence, KY, US: Taylor & Frances/Routledge.
Simpson, A., & Laham, S. M. (2015). Different relational models underlie prototypical left and right positions on social issues : Relational models, ideology, and social issues European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(2), 204 - 217. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2074
Sood, S., & Forehand, M. (2005). On self-referencing differences in judgment and choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 98, 144–154. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.05.005
Starcke, K., Polzer, C., Wolf, O. T., & Brand, M. (2011). Does stress alter everyday moral decision-making? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 210-219. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.010
Steffensmeier, D., & Britt, C. L. (2001). Judges′ Race and Judicial Decision Making: Do Black Judges Sentence Differently? . Social Science Quarterly, 82(4), 749 - 764. doi: 10.1111/0038-4941.00057
Szekely, R. D., Opre, A., & Miu, A. C. (2015). Religiosity enhances emotion and deontological choice in moral dilemmas. Personality and Individual Differences, 79, 104-109.
Tassy, S., Deruelle, C., Mancini, J., Leistedt, S., & Wicker, B. (2013). High levels of psychopathic traits alters moral choice but not moral judgment. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(229). doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00229
Tassy, S., Oullier, O., Duclos, Y., Coulon, O., Mancini, J., Deruelle, C., . . . Wicker, B. (2012). Disrupting the right prefrontal cortex alters moral judgement. SCAN, 7, 282-288. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr008
Tassy, S., Oullier, O., Mancini, J., & Wicker, B. (2013). Discrepancies between judgment and choice of action in moral dilemmas. Frontiers in Psychology 4(250). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00250
Thomson, J. J. (1985). The Trolley Problem. The Yale Law Journal, 94(6), 1395-1415.
Tong, E. M. W., & Yang, Z. (2011). Moral Hypocrisy: Of Proud and Grateful People. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(2), 159-165. doi: 10.1177/1948550610385711
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts. Psychological Review, 96(3), 506-520. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.96.3.506
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention: Cambridge University Press.
Turiel, E. (2008). Child and Adolescent Development: An Advanced Course: John Wiley & Sons.
Turiel, E., Hildebrandt, C., Wainryb, C., & Saltzstein, H. D. (1991). Judging Social Issues: Difficulties, Inconsistencies, and Consistencies Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 55(2), i+iii+v-vi+1-116. doi: 10.2307/1166056
Vauclair, C.-M., & Fischer, R. (2011). Do cultural values predict individuals’ moral attitudes? A cross-cultural
multilevel approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 645-657.
Vauclair, C.-M., Fischer, R., Ferreira, M. C., Guerra, V., Hößler, U., Karabati, S., . . . Spieß, E. (2014). What Kinds of Value Motives Guide People in Their Moral Attitudes? The Role of Personal and Prescriptive Values at the Culture Level and Individual Level Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(2), 211-228. doi: 10.1177/0022022114557487
Wang, R., Hempton, B. M., Dugan, J. P., & Komives, S. R. M. (2008). Cultural Differences: Why Do Asians Avoid Extreme Responses? Survey Practice, 1(3), 1-7. doi: 10.29115/SP-2008-0011
Youssef, F. F., Dookeeram, K., Basdeo, V., Francis, E., Doman, M., Mamed, D., . . . Legall, G. (2012). Stress alters personal moral decision making. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(4), 491 - 498. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.017
Zhong, C.-B., & Liljenquis, K. (2006). Washing Away Your Sins: ThreatenedMorality and Physical Cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 1451 - 1452. doi: 10.1126/science.1130726
Zweigenhaft, R. L., Phillips, B., Adams, K., Morse, C., & Horan, A. E. (1985). Religious preference and belief in a just world. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs, 111(3), 331-348.
方永泉. (2005). 從次文化研究到後次文化研究--談西方次文化研究的演變及其在教育上的啟示. 中等教育, 24-47.
沈六. (2002). 道德發展的家庭脈絡因素. 公民訓育學報, 11, 1-38. doi: 10.6231/CME.2002(11)01
洪泉湖. (2008). 台灣的多元文化. 台灣 五南.
陳舜文, & 邱振訓. (2011 ). 道德判斷的「雙重歷程模式」:認知、情緒與文化的整合觀點. 本土心理學研究, 36 33~76. doi: dx.doi.org/10.6254/2011.36.33
陸洛. (2007). 個人取向與社會取向的自我觀:概念分析與實徵測量. US-China Education Review, 4(2), 1-23.
指導教授 段正仁 審核日期 2020-1-20
推文 facebook   plurk   twitter   funp   google   live   udn   HD   myshare   reddit   netvibes   friend   youpush   delicious   baidu   
網路書籤 Google bookmarks   del.icio.us   hemidemi   myshare   

若有論文相關問題,請聯絡國立中央大學圖書館推廣服務組 TEL:(03)422-7151轉57407,或E-mail聯絡  - 隱私權政策聲明