博碩士論文 972207009 詳細資訊




以作者查詢圖書館館藏 以作者查詢臺灣博碩士 以作者查詢全國書目 勘誤回報 、線上人數:85 、訪客IP:3.128.203.143
姓名 李冠慧(Kuan-Hui Li)  查詢紙本館藏   畢業系所 認知與神經科學研究所
論文名稱 學齡前階段孩童眼動抑制能力的發展和特性
(The development and the characteristics of oculomotor inhibition in preschool children)
相關論文
★ 時間及空間對注意力暫失的影響 以及其可能的神經生理機制★ 注意力分配及眼球運動準備歷程對於眼動潛伏時間與眼動軌跡的影響
★ 注意力暫失中的數字表徵: 數字距離對注意力暫失的影響★ 利用跨顱磁刺激探討主動式注意力攫取的神經機制
★ 以數學模型及跨顱磁刺激探討注意力分配及眼球運動準備歷程★ 學齡前兒童之視覺注意力發展及電腦化注意力訓練效果之探討
★ 以跨顱磁刺激探討左側下部頂葉以及左側上部頂葉的功能在中文處理中所扮演的角色★ 性侵害犯的衝動行為表現-情緒狀態如何影響性侵害犯的抑制能力?
★ 學齡前階段孩童衝突解決和動作反應抑制能力的發展★ 6歲孩童與成人在數字和具體數量上的自動化處理
★ 期望效果之影響與可能的神經機制★ Attentional reorienting: the dynamic interaction between goal-directed and stimulus-driven attentioinal control
★ 前額葉眼動區在視覺搜尋作業上對不同干擾物特徵與顯示時間扮演的角色★ Roles of the Pre-supplementary Motor Area and Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus in Stimulus Selective Stop-signal task: A Theta Burst Transcranial!Magnetic! Stimulation!Study
★ Investigation of posterior parietal cortex visuospatial control over processing in near and far space using transcranial magnetic stimulation★ Using Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation to Investigate the Roles of the Dorsal Lateral Prefrontal Cortex and the Temporoparietal Junction in Top-Down and Bottom-Up Conflict Resolution
檔案 [Endnote RIS 格式]    [Bibtex 格式]    [相關文章]   [文章引用]   [完整記錄]   [館藏目錄]   [檢視]  [下載]
  1. 本電子論文使用權限為同意立即開放。
  2. 已達開放權限電子全文僅授權使用者為學術研究之目的,進行個人非營利性質之檢索、閱讀、列印。
  3. 請遵守中華民國著作權法之相關規定,切勿任意重製、散佈、改作、轉貼、播送,以免觸法。

摘要(中) 本論文指稱的「學齡前階段」,特指4~6歲兒童開始在幼稚園接受大量認知學習的時期。過去研究顯示,執行功能在學齡前階段有顯著的進展。眼動抑制能力亦是執行功能的一環,卻鮮少有研究探討學齡前兒童眼動抑制能力的發展和特性。每天從視覺輸入的訊息很多,兒童究竟如何抑制不必要的眼動,進而將注意力放在當下的學習上,眼動抑制能力皆扮演一個重要的角色。發展性研究顯示,眼動抑制能力會隨著發展而有顯著進步,直到青少年階段前額葉功能發展成熟後才停止。在成人研究上也顯示,眼動抑制能力除了會受到年齡因素調控外,目標出現類型的機率和視覺標的物等,亦會調節受試者的眼動抑制表現。另一方面,相關性研究亦指出,眼動抑制能力和智力及執行功能 (如:衝動控制和衝突解決能力) 皆有顯著的高度相關。然而學齡前兒童的眼動抑制能力是否會受到這些特性的影響,目前尚未有通盤的瞭解。
本研究將以順向和反向眼動跳視作業量化兒童的眼動抑制表現,並以不同的實驗操弄探究兒童眼動抑制的特性。本研究共有四個實驗:(1)實驗一操弄順向和反向眼動類型的機率,探究機率如何調控兒童的抑制表現。(2)實驗二操弄視覺標的物的有無,探究視覺標的物如何影響兒童的抑制能力。另外,實驗二亦將42位兒童分成4歲、5歲和6歲組,以瞭解學齡前階段兒童的眼動抑制發展曲線;並收集13位成人的抑制表現,以比較學齡前兒童和成人的表現差異。(3)實驗三驗證競爭模型和注意力再次攫取假說,探討哪一個假說能解釋視覺標的物對抑制表現的影響歷程。(4)實驗四比較42位孩童在五項作業表現之間的相關性,包括:順向和反向眼動跳視作業、停止信號作業、注意力網絡作業、瑞文氏圖型推理測驗、魏氏學齡前兒童智力測驗。
本研究發現,眼動抑制能力在學齡前階段亦有顯著的進步。學齡前兒童的眼動抑制表現亦會受到目標類型機率和視覺標的物的影響,當反向眼動跳視出現的機率越高,或加上一個視覺標的物在目標出現的相反位置上,皆能增進兒童抑制表現的正確率並減少抑制所需的反應時間。另一方面,眼動抑制能力亦和智力及衝動控制能力有顯著的高度相關,智力分數越高或衝動控制表現越好的兒童,越可能有較佳的眼動抑制表現。
摘要(英) In this study, children among the ages of four to six years were called preschool stage. At preschool stage, children usually went to kindergarten and began to learn lots of knowledge. Past studies have indicated that executive function has a significant improvement in preschool period. Oculomotor inhibition is one of the crucial abilities of executive function, but few studies have investigated the development and the characteristics of oculomotor inhibition in preschool children. A vast amount of information is received through visual pathways every day. Oculomotor inhibition plays an important role in suppressing an unnecessary eye-movement, and concentrating on learning. Many developmental studies have indicated that oculomotor inhibition will improve with age until the prefrontal lobe function becomes mature in adolescence. Moreover, this ability may be modulated by several factors, such as trial type probability and visual landmark etc. Oculomotor inhibition also has reported to have a significant correlation with intelligence or other executive functions, including impulsive control and conflict resolution. However, how these factors influencing oculomotor inhibition in preschool children are still unclear.
In this study, a version of children pro-/anti-saccade task was conducted to gauge the development of oculomotor inhibition, and several factors were manipulated to investigate the characteristics of oculomotor inhibition in preschool children. There were four experiments: (1) Three trial type probabilities were manipulated to investigate how this factor modulating inhibitory control. (2) A visual landmark was added to examine how this factor benefits the inhibitory control. Furthermore, four age groups, including 4y, 5y, 6y and young adults, were recruited to understand the developmental changes of oculomotor inhibition and to compare the inhibitory differences between preschool children and adults. (3) Two hypotheses of competition model and attentional re-allocation were testified to investigate which one can explain the inhibitory mechanism when adding a visual landmark. (4) Two correlational analyses were conducted to understand the relationships between oculomotor inhibition, impulsive control (measured by stop-signal task) and conflict resolution (measured by attentional network task), and to investigate the associations between oculomotor inhibition and intelligence (measured by WPPSI-R and Ravens).
We found that oculomotor inhibition has a gradual but significant development in preschool period. This ability may be improved when there are higher probability of anti-saccade trials or when adding a visual landmark in the opposite direction of the target. Moreover, oculomotor inhibition has significant correlations with intelligence, especially with fluid intelligence, and with impulsive control. Children with higher intelligence quotient scores or with better impulsive control may have better oculomotor inhibition in preschool period.
關鍵字(中) ★ 發展
★ 學齡前孩童
★ 眼動抑制
關鍵字(英) ★ preschool children
★ oculomotor inhibition
★ development
論文目次 Chapter 1: Introduction 1
1.1 Attention and executive function 1
1.2 Oculomotor inhibition and its importance 3
1.3 Pro-/anti-saccade task and two inhibitory indexes 4
1.4 The necessity of the current study 6
1.5 Purposes of research 7
Chapter 2: Literature review 10
2.1 Development of oculomotor inhibition 10
2.2 Characteristics of oculomotor inhibition 17
2.2.1 Trial type probability 22
2.2.2 Visual landmarks 24
2.2.3 Intelligence 27
2.2.4 Other executive functions 28
Chapter 3: Experiment 1 30
3.1 Method 30
3.2 Results 35
3.3 Discussion 45
Chapter 4: Experiment 2 49
4.1 Experiment 2-1 49
4.1.1 Method 49
4.1.2 Results 51
4.2 Experiment 2-2 56
4.2.1 Method 56
4.2.2 Results 57
4.3 Discussion 64
Chapter 5: Experiment 3 67
5.1 Experiment 3-1: The discriminative task 68
5.1.1 Methods 68
5.1.2 Results 71
5.2 Experiment 3-2: The cueing task 74
5.2.1 Methods 74
5.2.2 Results 78
5.3 Discussion 87
Chapter 6: Experiment 4 89
6.1 Method 89
6.2 Results 94
6.3 Discussion 99
Chapter 7: General Discussion and Conclusion 103
7.1 Summary of research findings 103
7.2 Limitation of the study 109
7.3 Recommendations for future research 109
References 110
參考文獻 Agostino, A., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2010).
Executive functions underlying multiplicative
reasoning: Problem type matters. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 105(4), 286-305.
Ardila, A., Pineda, D., & Rosselli, M. (2000). Correlation
between intelligence test scores and executive
function measures. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 15(1), 31-36.
Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in
cognitive control. The Neuroscientist, 13(3), 214-228.
Biscaldi, M., Fischer, B., & Hartnegg, K. (2000).
Voluntary saccadic control in dyslexia. Perception, 29
(5), 509-521.
Briand, K. A., Strallow, D., Hening, W., Poizner, H., &
Sereno, A. B. (1999). Control of voluntary and
reflexive saccades in Parkinson's disease.
Experimental Brain Research, 129(1), 38-48.
Bruno, A., Brambati, S. M., Perani, D., & Morrone, M. C.
(2006). Development of saccadic suppression in
children. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(3), 1011-1017.
Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a
predictor of children's mathematics ability:
Inhibition, switching, and working memory.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.
Chiau, H.-Y., Tseng, P., Su, J.-H., Hsu, W.-Y., Tzeng, O.
J. L., Hung, D. L., et al. (submitted). Trial type
probability modulates the cost of anti-saccades.
Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive
functions. In E. Bialystok & F. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan
cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 70-95). NY:
Oxford University Press.
Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2007). Inhibition of
action, thought, and emotion: A selective
neurobiological review. Journal of the American
Association of Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12
(3), 99-114.
Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., &
Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe:
The organization of goal-directed behavior. Cognitive
Psychology, 30(3), 257-303.
Eenshuistra, R. M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Weidema, M. A., &
van der Molen, M. W. (2007). Developmental changes in
oculomotor control and working-memory efficiency. Acta
Psychologica, 124(1), 139-158.
Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M.
M., Hamby, A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution
of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills
in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology,
26(1), 465-486.
Evdokimidis, I., Smyrnis, N., Constantinidis, T. S.,
Stefanis, N. C., Avramopoulos, D., Paximadis, C., et
al. (2002). The anti-saccade task in a sample of 2,006
young men. I. Normal population characteristics.
Experimental Brain Research, 147(1), 45-52.
Everling, S., Dorris, M. C., Klein, R. M., & Munoz, D. P.
(1999). Role of primate superior colliculus in
preparation and execution of anti-saccades and pro-
saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(7), 2740-2754.
Everling, S., & Fischer, B. (1998). The antisaccade: A
review of basic research and clinical studies.
Neuropsychologia, 36(9), 885-899.
Everling, S., & Munoz, D. P. (2000). Neuronal correlates
for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and
anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field.
Journal of Neuroscience, 20(1), 387-400.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I.,
& Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional
networks. Neuroimage, 26(2), 471-479.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner,
M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence
of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 14(3), 340-347.
Fan, J., & Posner, M. (2004). Human attentional networks.
Psychiatrische Praxis, 31 Suppl 2, S210-214.
Fischer, B., Biscaldi, M., & Gezeck, S. (1997). On the
development of voluntary and reflexive components in
human saccade generation. Brain Research, 754, 285-297.
Fischer, B., Biscaldi, M., & Otto, P. (1993). Saccadic eye
movements of dyslexic adult subjects.
Neuropsychologia, 31(9), 887-906.
Frank, M. C., Vul, E., & Johnson, S. P. (2009).
Development of infants' attention to faces during the
first year. Cognition, 110(2), 160-170.
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among
inhibition and interference control functions: A
latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 133(1), 101-135.
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E.,
Defries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all
executive functions are related to intelligence.
Psychological Science, 17(2), 172-179.
Fukushima, J., Hatta, T., & Fukushima, K. (2000).
Development of voluntary control of saccadic eye
movements. I. Age-related changes in normal children.
Brain & development, 22(3), 173-180.
Geier, C. F., Terwilliger, R., Teslovich, T., Velanova,
K., & Luna, B. (2009). Immaturities in reward
processing and its influence on inhibitory control in
adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1613-1629.
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O.,
Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al.
(1999). Brain development during childhood and
adolescence: A developmental MRI study. Nature
Neuroscience, 2(10), 861-863.
Gmeindl, L., Rontal, A., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2005).
Strategic modulation of the fixation-offset effect:
Dissociable effects of target probability on pro-
saccades and anti-saccades. Experimental Brain
Research, 164(2), 194-204.
Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2002). Programming of
endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a
competitive integration model. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(5),
1039-1054.
Grossmann, T., Striano, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2007).
Developmental changes in infants' processing of happy
and angry facial expressions: A neurobehavioral study.
Brain and Cognition, 64(1), 30-41.
Hanisch, C., Radach, R., Holtkamp, K., Herpertz-Dahlmann,
B., & Konrad, K. (2006). Oculomotor inhibition in
children with and without attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Neural
Transmission, 113(5), 671-684.
Irving, E. L., Tajik-Parvinchi, D. J., Lillakas, L.,
Gonzalez, E. G., & Steinbach, M. J. (2009). Mixed pro-
and anti-saccade performance in children and adults.
Brain Research, 1255, 67-74.
Johnson, M. H. (1995). The inhibition of automatic
saccades in early infancy. Developmental
Psychobiology, 28, 281–291.
Juan, C. H., Muggleton, N. G., Tzeng, O. J., Hung, D. L.,
Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2008). Segregation of visual
selection and saccades in human frontal eye fields.
Cerebral Cortex, 18(10), 2410-2415.
Karatekin, C. (2006). Improving antisaccade performance in
adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD). Experimental Brain Research, 174(2),
324-341.
Karatekin, C., Bingham, C., & White, T. (2009). Oculomotor
and pupillometric indices of pro- and anti-saccade
performance in youth-onset psychosis and attention
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin.
Klein, C., & Foerster, F. (2001). Development of pro-
saccade and anti-saccade task performance in
participants aged 6 to 26 years. Psychophysiology, 38
(2), 179-189.
Klein, C. H., Raschke, A., & Brandenbusch, A. (2003).
Development of pro- and anti-saccades in children with
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and
healthy controls. Psychophysiology, 40(1), 17-28.
Kramer, A. F., Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. M., & Cassavaugh,
N. D. (2005). Development of attentional and
oculomotor control. Developmental Psychology, 41(5),
760-772.
Kristjansson, A., Chen, Y., & Nakayama, K. (2001). Less
attention is more in the preparation of anti-saccades,
but not pro-saccades. Nature Neuroscience, 4(10), 1037-
1042.
Kristjansson, A., Vandenbroucke, M. W., & Driver, J.
(2004). When pros become cons for anti- versus pro-
saccades: Factors with opposite or common effects on
different saccade types. Experimental Brain Research,
155(2), 231-244.
Liu, C. L., Chiau, H. Y., Tseng, P., Hung, D. L., Tzeng,
O. J., Muggleton, N. G., et al. (2010). Anti-saccade
cost is modulated by contextual experience of location
probability. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(3), 1438-
1447.
Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Munoz, D. P., Merriam, E. P.,
Garver, K. E., Minshew, N. J., et al. (2001).
Maturation of widely distributed brain function sub-
serves cognitive development. Neuroimage, 13(5), 786-
793.
Mahone, E. M., Mostofsky, S. H., Lasker, A. G., Zee, D., &
Denckla, M. B. (2009). Oculomotor anomalies in
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for
deficits in response preparation and inhibition.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(7), 749-756.
Massen, C. (2004). Parallel programming of exogenous and
endogenous components in the anti-saccade task. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3),
475-498.
McNamee, R. L., Dunfee, K. L., Luna, B., Clark, D. B.,
Eddy, W. F., & Tarter, R. E. (2008). Brain activation,
response inhibition, and increased risk for substance
use disorder. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
Research, 32(3), 405-413.
Michel, F., & Anderson, M. (2009). Using the anti-saccade
task to investigate the relationship between the
development of inhibition and the development of
intelligence. Developmental Science, 12(2), 272-288.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A.
H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and
diversity of executive functions and their
contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: A
latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1),
49-100.
Mokler, A., & Fischer, B. (1999). The recognition and
correction of involuntary pro-saccades in an anti-
saccade task. Experimental Brain Research, 125(4), 511-
516.
Munoz, D. P., Armstrong, I. T., Hampton, K. A., & Moore,
K. D. (2003). Altered control of visual fixation and
saccadic eye movements in attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90
(1), 503-514.
Munoz, D. P., Broughton, J. R., Goldring, J. E., &
Armstrong, I. T. (1998). Age-related performance of
human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks.
Experimental Brain Research, 121, 391-400.
Munoz, D. P., & Everling, S. (2004). Look away: The anti-
saccade task and the voluntary control of eye
movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(3), 218-228.
Nakagawa, A., & Sukigara, M. (2007). Infant eye and head
movements toward the side opposite the cue in the anti-
saccade paradigm. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(5).
O'Driscoll, G. A., Alpert, N. M., Matthysse, S. W., Levy,
D. L., Rauch, S. L., & Holzman, P. S. (1995).
Functional neuroanatomy of anti-saccade eye movements
investigated with positron emission tomography.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America, 92(3), 925-929.
O'Driscoll, G. A., Depatie, L., Holahan, A. L., Savion-
Lemieux, T., Barr, R. G., Jolicoeur, C., et al.
(2005). Executive functions and methylphenidate
response in subtypes of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry,
57(11), 1452-1460.
Olk, B., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Why are anti-saccades
slower than pro-saccades? A novel finding using a new
paradigm. NeuroReport, 14(1), 151-155.
Park, J., Shimojo, E., & Shimojo, S. (2010). Roles of
familiarity and novelty in visual preference judgments
are segregated across object categories. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 107(33), 14552-14555.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3-
25.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1980). The development
of attentional mechanisms. Nebraska Symposium on
Motivation, 28, 1-52.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Educating the
human brain. Washington DC:American Psychological
Association.
Reuter, B., Rakusan, L., & Kathmanna, N. (2005). Poor anti-
saccade performance in schizophrenia: An inhibition
deficit? Psychiatry Research, 135(1), 1-10.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D.,
Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., et al. (2004).
Development of attentional networks in childhood.
Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029-1040.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Davis-
Stober, C. P. (2004). Development of the time course
for processing conflict: An event-related potentials
study with 4 year olds and adults. BMC neuroscience,
5, 39.
Scerif, G., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Campos, R., Elsabbagh,
M., Driver, J., & Cornish, K. (2005). To look or not
to look? Typical and atypical development of
oculomotor control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
17(4), 591-604.
Smyrnis, N., Evdokimidis, I., Stefanis, N. C.,
Constantinidis, T. S., Avramopoulos, D., Theleritis,
C., et al. (2002). The anti-saccade task in a sample
of 2,006 young males. II. Effects of task parameters.
Experimental Brain Research, 147(1), 53-63.
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome,
S. E., Kan, E., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Developmental
mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in
normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(38), 8223-
8231.
St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006).
Executive functions and achievements in school:
Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory.
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59
(4), 745-759.
Thatcher, R., Walker, R., & Giudice, S. (1987). Human
cerebral hemispheres develop at different rates and
ages. Science, 236(4805), 1110-1113.
Thatcher, R. W., North, D. M., & Biver, C. J. (2008).
Development of cortical connections as measured by EEG
coherence and phase delays. Human Brain Mapping, 29
(12), 1400-1415.
Tillman, C. M., Thorell, L. B., Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin,
G. (2008). Motor response inhibition and execution in
the stop-signal task: Development and relation to ADHD
behaviors. Child Neuropsychology, 14(1), 42-59.
Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). The stochastic
modeling of elementary psychological processes.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Towse, J. N., Lewis, C., & Knowles, M. (2007). When
knowledge is not enough: The phenomenon of goal
neglect in preschool children. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 96(4), 320-332.
Towse, J. N., Redbond, J., Houston-Price, C. M. T., &
Cook, S. (2000). Understanding the dimensional change
card sort: Perspectives from task success and failure.
Cognitive Development, 15(3), 347-365.
指導教授 阮啟弘(Chi-Hung Juan) 審核日期 2010-10-19
推文 facebook   plurk   twitter   funp   google   live   udn   HD   myshare   reddit   netvibes   friend   youpush   delicious   baidu   
網路書籤 Google bookmarks   del.icio.us   hemidemi   myshare   

若有論文相關問題,請聯絡國立中央大學圖書館推廣服務組 TEL:(03)422-7151轉57407,或E-mail聯絡  - 隱私權政策聲明