參考文獻 |
Reference
Ahissar, M. (2007). Dyslexia and the anchoring-deficit hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 458–465. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.015
Ahissar, M., Protopapas, A., Reid, M., & Merzenich, M. M. (2000). Auditory processing parallels reading abilities in adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(12), 6832.
Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I. C. (2011). Statistical learning in typically developing children: the role of age and speed of stimulus presentation. Developmental Science, 14(3), 464–473. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00937.x
Aslin, R. N., Saffran, J. R., & Newport, E. L. (1998). Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 9(4), 321–324.
Banai, K., & Ahissar, M. (2005). Auditory Processing Deficits in Dyslexia: Task or Stimulus Related? Cerebral Cortex, 16(12), 1718–1728. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj107
Banai, K., & Ahissar, M. (2010). On the importance of anchoring and the consequences of its impairment in dyslexia. (A. Fawcett & S. Vicari, Eds.)Dyslexia, 16(3), 240–257. doi:10.1002/dys.407
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271(5647), 746–747.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature.
Chow, B. W.-Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Burgess, S. (2005). Phonological Processing Skills and Early Reading Abilities in Hong Kong Chinese Kindergarteners Learning to Read English as a Second Language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(1), 81–87. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.1.81
Denckla, M. B., & Cutting, L. E. (1999). History and significance of rapid automatized naming. Annals of Dyslexia, 49(1), 29–42.
Evans, J. L., Saffran, J. R., & Robe-Torres, K. (2009). Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 321.
Frost, R., Siegelman, N, Narkiss, A., & Afek, L. (in press). What predicts successful literacy acquisition in a second language?Psychological Science.
Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G. J., Adams, A. M., & Martin, A. J. (1999). Phonological short‐term memory and vocabulary development: further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13(1), 65–77.
Gleitman, L. R. (1985). Orthographic Resources Affect Reading Acquisition--If They Are Used. Remedial and Special Education, 6(6), 24–36. doi:10.1177/074193258500600606
Graf Estes, K., Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., & Saffran, J. R. (2007). Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological Science, 18(3), 254–260. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01885.x
Gómez, R. L., & Gerken, L. (1998). Determining the basis of abstraction in artificial language acquisition. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 434.
Ho, C. S. H., Law, T. P. S., & Ng, P. M. (2000). The phonological deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 13(1), 57–79.
Ho, C. S., & Bryant, P. (1997). Development of phonological awareness of Chinese children in Hong Kong. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26(1), 109–126.
Hu, C. F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The Role of Phonological Processing in Early Reading Ability: What We Can Learn From Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2(1), 55–79.
Huang, H. S., & Hanley, J. R. (1995). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition, 54(1), 73–98.
Koyama, M. S., Hansen, P. C., & Stein, J. F. (2008). Logographic Kanji versus Phonographic Kana in Literacy Acquisition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145(1), 41–55. doi:10.1196/annals.1416.005
Koyama, M. S., Stein, J. F., Stoodley, C. J., & Hansen, P. C. (2010). Functional MRI evidence for the importance of visual short-term memory in logographic reading. European Journal of Neuroscience, 33(3), 539–548. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07534.x
Lee, C.-Y. (2009). The Cognitive and neural basis for learning to read Chinese. Journal of Basic Education, 18 (2) 63-85.
Lee, C.-Y., Tsai, J.-L., Huang, H.-W., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). The temporal signatures of semantic and phonological activations for Chinese sublexical processing: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1121(1), 150–159. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.117
Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O.-P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 263–284.
McArthur, G. M., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Frequency discrimination deficits in people with specific language impairment: reliability, validity, and linguistic correlates. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(3), 527–541. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2004/041)
McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2000). Developmental issues in Chinese children’s character acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 50–55. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.92.1.50
McBride-Chang, C., & Manis, F. R. (1996). Structural invariance in the associations of naming speed, phonological awareness, and verbal reasoning in good and poor readers: A test of the double deficit hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8(4), 323–339.
McBride-Chang, C., Bialystok, E., Chong, K. K. Y., & Li, Y. (2004). Levels of phonological awareness in three cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(2), 93–111. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2004.05.001
McBride-Chang, C., Chow, B. W. Y., Zhong, Y., Burgess, S., & Hayward, W. G. (2005). Chinese character acquisition and visual skills in two Chinese scripts. Reading and Writing, 18(2), 99–128. doi:10.1007/s11145-004-7343-5
Meng, X., Sai, X., Wang, C., Wang, J., Sha, S., & Zhou, X. (2005). Auditory and speech processing and reading development in Chinese school children: behavioural and ERP evidence. Dyslexia, 11(4), 292–310. doi:10.1002/dys.309
Nahum, M., Daikhin, L., Lubin, Y., Cohen, Y., & Ahissar, M. (2010). From Comparison to Classification: A Cortical Tool for Boosting Perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(3), 1128–1136. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1781-09.2010
Neuhaus, G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of Information Processing in Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) and Their Relation to Reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78(4), 359–373. doi:10.1006/jecp.2000.2576
Pelucchi, B., Hay, J.F., Saffran, J.R. (2009). Learning in reverse: Eight-month-old infants track backwards transitional probabilities. Cognition, 113, 244-247.
Perruchet, P., & Pacton, S. (2006). Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 233-238.
Saffran, J. R. (2003). Statistical language learning: Mechanisms and constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 110-114.
Saffran, J.R., Aslin, R.N., & Newport, E.L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-olds. Science, 274, 1926–1928.
Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70(1), 27–52.
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (1996). Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(4), 606–621.
Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P. (2001). The role of phonological awareness and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 37(6), 1–155.
Tallal, P. (1980). Language and reading: Some perceptual prerequisites. Annals of Dyslexia, 30(1), 170–178.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(5), 276–86– discussion 287–91.
Turk-Browne, N. B., Jungé, J. A., & Scholl, B. J. (2005). The Automaticity of Visual Statistical Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(4), 552–564. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.552
Tzeng, Y.-L., & Lee, C.-Y. (2012). The developmental trajectories of orthography-to-phonology mapping consistency in learning to read Chinese. Contemporary Educational Research Quarterly, 20(4).
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 1–21.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73-87.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468.
Wang, C. C., Hung, L. Y., Chang, Y. W., & Chen, H. F. (2010). 一到九年級學生國字識字量發展 Number of Characters School Students Know from Grade 1 to 9. 教育心理學報.
Wechsler, D. (2008a). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition.
San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
Witton, C., Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P.C., Richardson, A. J., Griffiths, T. D., Rees, A., et al. (1998). Sensitivity to dynamic auditory and visual stimuli predicts nonword reading ability in both dyslexic and normal readers. Current Biology, 8(14), 791–797.
Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 123–141.
Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 988–1000.
李佳穎,曾郁琳,林宛萱(2010)。以事件誘發電位探討閱讀障礙學童與一般學童對構字規則之掌握。2010年台灣學障學會年會暨學術研討會論文。台中:台灣。
李俊仁(2009)。閱讀研究議題二:從識字及詞彙量建立文本可讀性分析指標。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(編號:100-2420-H-003-002-MY2),未出版。
曾世杰(2004)。聲韻覺識、唸名速度與中文閱讀障礙。台北:心理出版社。
俞筱鈞(1993)。瑞文氏圖形推理測驗系列。 台北:中國行為科學社。
|