參考文獻 |
[1] Alekseeva, M., Myachykov, A., Bermudez-Margaretto, B., & Shtyrov, Y. (2024). Morphosyntactic prediction in automatic neural processing of spoken language: EEG evidence. Brain Research, 1836, 148949.
[2] Allen, R. J., Waterman, A. H., Yang, T. X., & Jaroslawska, A. J. (2023). Working memory in action: Remembering and following instructions. In R. H. Logie, Z.
Wen, S. E. Gathercole, N. Cowan, and R. W. Engle (eds.), Memory in science for society: There is nothing as practical as a good theory. Oxford University Press.
[3] Amberber, M., Baker, B., & Harvey, M. (Eds.). (2010). Complex predicates: Crosslinguistic perspectives on event structure. Cambridge University Press.
[4] Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. The psychology of learning and motivation, 8, 47–89.
[5] Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11) 417-423.
[6] Baggio, G., & Hagoort, P. (2011). The balance between memory and unification in semantics: A dynamic account of the N400. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(9), 1338-1367.
[7] Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2005). Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 17(1), 137-153.
[8] Belyk, M., & Brown, S. (2014). Perception of affective and linguistic prosody: an ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 9(9), 1395-1403.
[9] Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkanen, L. (2011). Simple composition: A magnetoencephalography investigation into the comprehension of minimal linguistic phrases. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(8), 2801-2814.
[10] Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal statistical society: series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289-300.
[11] Blake, B. J. (2001). Case. Cambridge University Press.
[12] Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders. John Wiley & Sons.
[13] Bochner, S., & Jones, J. (2008). Child language development: Learning to talk (2th ed.). John Wiley & Sons.
[14] Bornkessel, I. (2002). The argument dependency model: A neurocognitive approach to incremental interpretation [Doctoral dissertation, Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience Leipzig].
[15] Bornkessel, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2006). The extended argument dependency model: a neurocognitive approach to sentence comprehension across languages.
Psychological review, 113(4), 787.
[16] Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2008). An alternative perspective on
“semantic P600”effects in language comprehension. Brain research reviews, 59(1), 55-73.
[17] Boustani, N., Pishghadam, R., & Shayesteh, S. (2021). Multisensory input modulates P200 and L2 sentence comprehension: a one-week consolidation phase. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 746813.
[18] Brennan, J., & Pylkkanen, L. (2012). The time-course and spatial distribution of brain activity associated with sentence processing. Neuroimage, 60(2), 1139-1148.
[19] Brodhun, C., Borelli, E., & Weiss, T. (2024). Neural correlates of word processing influenced by painful primes. Plos one, 19(1), e0295148.
[20] Brouwer, H., Fitz, H., & Hoeks, J. C. J. (2012). Getting real about semantic illusions: rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension.
Brain Research, 1446, 127–143.
[21] Brouwer, H., & Hoeks, J. C. (2013). A time and place for language comprehension: mapping the N400 and the P600 to a minimal cortical network. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 7, 758.
[22] Brouwer, H., Crocker, M. W., Venhuizen, N. J., & Hoeks, J. C. (2017). A neurocomputational model of the N400 and the P600 in language processing. Cognitive science, 41, 1318-1352.
[23] Buchsbaum, B. R. (2010). Neural Basis of Working Memory. In George F. Koob,
Michel Le Moal & Richard F. Thompson (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience(pp. 334-341). Elsevier Science.
[24] Buchsbaum, B. R. (2015). Working Memory and Language. In G. Hickok & S. L. Small (Eds.), Neurobiology of Language (pp. 863-875). Academic Press.
[25] Burkhardt, P. (2007). The P600 reflects cost of new information in discourse memory. Neuroreport, 18(17), 1851-1854.
[26] Casenhiser, D., & Bencini, G. M. (2015). Argument structure constructions. In Dabrowska, E., & Divjak, D. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 296-322).
[27] Chao, Y. R. (1968). A Grammar of Spoken Chinese. University of California Press.
[28] Chai, W. J., Abd Hamid, A. I., & Abdullah, J. M. (2018). Working memory from the psychological and neurosciences perspectives: a review. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 401.
[29] Chinese Knowledge and Information Processing Lab (1993). Jishu baogao no. 93-05: zhongwen cilei fenxi [Technical Report no. 93-05: Part-of-speech analysis] (3rd ed.) Institute of Information Science, Academia Sinica.
[30] Chinese Knowledge and Information Processing Lab (2015). Technical Report no. 15-01: Semantic Roles and Semantic Role Labeling Institute of Information Science, Academia Sinica.
[31] Chomsky, N. (1956). Three models for the description of language. IRE Transactions on information theory, 2(3), 113-124.
[32] Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. Mouton.
[33] Chung, R. F. (2006). Dangdai yuyanxue gailun [Introduction to Modern Linguistics]. Wu-Nan Culture Enterprise.
[34] Ciaccio, L. A., Burki, A., & Clahsen, H. (2023). Inter-individual variability in morphological processing: An ERP study on German plurals. Journal of Neurolinguistics,
67, 101138.
[35] Conlen, M. M. (2016). A Linguistic Comparison: Stress-timed and syllable-timed languages and their impact on second language acquisition. [Honors College Thesis,
Classical and Modern Languages, Literatures, and Cultures]. Wayne State University.
[36] Coulson, S., King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1998). Expect the unexpected: Event-related
brain response to morphosyntactic violations. Language and cognitive processes, 13(1), 21-58.
[37] Crosson, B., Rodriguez, A. D., Copland, D., Fridriksson, J., Krishnamurthy, L. C., Meinzer, M., ... & Leff, A. P. (2019). Neuroplasticity and aphasia treatments: new approaches for an old problem. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 90(10), 1147-1155.
[38] Davidson, A., & Souza, P. (2024). Relationships Between Auditory Processing and
Cognitive Abilities in Adults: A Systematic Review. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 67(1), 296-345.
[39] Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dupoux, E., & Gout, A. (2000). Electrophysiological correlates of phonological processing: a cross-linguistic study. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 12(4), 635-647.
[40] Delogu, F., Brouwer, H., & Crocker, M. W. (2019). Event-related potentials index lexical retrieval (N400) and integration (P600) during language comprehension. Brain and cognition, 135, 103569.
[41] DeLong, K. A., & Kutas, M. (2016). Hemispheric differences and similarities in comprehending more and less predictable sentences. Neuropsychologia, 91, 380-393.
[42] Denes, P. B., & Pinson, E. N. (1973). The Speech Chain: The Physics and Biology of Spoken Language. Anchor Press.
[43] Denes, P. B., & Pinson, E. N. (2007). The Speech Chain: The Physics and Biology of Spoken Language (2nd ed.). W.H. Freeman and Company.
[44] Diesch, E., Eulitz, C., Hampson, S., & Ross, B. (1996). The neurotopography of vowels as mirrored by evoked magnetic field measurements. Brain and language,53(2), 143-168.
[45] Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., & Pylkkanen, L. (2009). Sensitivity to syntax in visual cortex. Cognition, 110(3), 293-321.
[46] Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., Farmer, T. A., & Pylkkanen, L. (2010). Early occipital sensitivity to syntactic category is based on form typically. Psychological Science, 21(5), 629-634.
[47] Duncan-Johnson, C. C., & Donchin, E. (1982). The P300 component of the eventrelated brain potential as an index of information processing. Biological psychology, 14(1-2), 1-52.
[48] Durrant, D., & Lovrinic , J. H. (1995). Bases of Hearing Sciences (3rd ed.). Williams & Wilkins.
[49] Federmeier, K. D., Wlotko, E. W., De Ochoa-Dewald, E., & Kutas, M. (2007).Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing. Brain research, 1146,75-84.
[50] Ferlazzo, F., Fagioli, S., Di Nocera, F., & Sdoia, S. (2008). Shifting attention across
near and far spaces: Implications for the use of hands-free cell phones while driving. Accident Analysis & Prevention 40 (6), 1859 1864.
[51] Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G., & Ferraro, V. (2002). Good-enough representations in language comprehension. Current directions in psychological science, 11(1), 11-15.
[52] Flick, G., Oseki, Y., Kaczmarek, A. R., Al Kaabi, M., Marantz, A., & Pylkkanen, L.(2018). Building words and phrases in the left temporal lobe. Cortex, 106, 213-236.
[53] Fox, A. (2002). Prosodic features and prosodic structure: The phonology of suprasegmentals. OUP Oxford.
[54] Friederici, A. D., Von Cramon, D. Y., & Kotz, S. A. (1999). Language related brain potentials in patients with cortical and subcortical left hemisphere lesions. Brain,122(6), 1033-1047.
[55] Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in cognitive sciences, 6(2), 78-84.
[56] Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A., & Saddy, D. (2002). Distinct neurophysiological patterns reflecting aspects of syntactic complexity and syntactic repair. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 31, 45-63.
[57] Friederici, A. D., & Kotz, S. A. (2003). The brain basis of syntactic processes:functional imaging and lesion studies. Neuroimage, 20, S8-S17.
[58] Friederici, A. D., Ruschemeyer, S. A., Hahne, A., & Fiebach, C. J. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension:localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral cortex, 13(2), 170-177.
[59] Friederici, A. D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R. I., & Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[60] Friederici, A. D., & Weissenborn, J. (2007). Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax–semantic interface. Brain research, 1146, 50-58.
[61] Friederici, A. D., & Wartenburger, I. (2010). Language and brain. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(2), 150-159.
[62] Friederici, A. D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological reviews, 91(4), 1357-1392.
[63] Friedrich, C. K. (2003). Prosody and spoken word recognition: Behavioral and ERP
correlates [Doctoral dissertation, Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience Leipzig].
[64] Frisch, S., Beim Graben, P., & Schlesewsky, M. (2004). Parallelizing grammatical functions: P600 and P345 reflect different cost of reanalysis. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 14(02), 531-549.
[65] Fry, D. B. (1955). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27(4), 765-768.
[66] Fry, D. B. (1958). Experiments in the perception of stress. Language and speech, 1(2), 126-152.
[67] Fujisaki, H., & Hirose, K. (1984). Analysis of voice fundamental frequency contours for declarative sentences of Japanese. Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan(E), 5(4), 233-242.
[68] Gasiorek, J., & Aune, R. Kelly. (2018). Message Processing: The Science of Creating Understanding. University of Hawaii Manoa.
[69] Geeraerts, D., & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.). (2007). The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics. OUP USA.
[70] Goldberg, A. E. (1992). Argument structure constructions. University of California, Berkeley.
[71] Gray, C. (2010). The New Social Story Book (10th Anniversary ed.). Future Horizons.
[72] Grober, E. H., Beardsley, W., & Caramazza, A. (1978). Parallel function strategy in pronoun assignment. Cognition, 6(2), 117-133.
[73] Gussenhoven, C. (2004). The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge University Press.
[74] Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and cognitive processes, 8(4), 439-483.
[75] Hagoort, P. (2008). The fractionation of spoken language understanding by measuring electrical and magnetic brain signals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 1055-1069.
[76] Hagoort, P., Baggio, G., & Willems, R. M. (2009). Semantic unification. In The cognitive neurosciences (4th ed.).(pp. 819-836). MIT press.
[77] Hall, S. (1980). ”Encoding / Decoding.” In: S. Hall, D. Hobson, A. Lowe, and P. Willis (eds). Culture, Media, Language: Working Papers in Cultural Studies, 1972–79. London: Hutchinson, pp. 128–138.
[78] Hamilton, L. S., Oganian, Y., Hall, J., & Chang, E. F. (2021). Parallel and distributed encoding of speech across human auditory cortex. Cell, 184(18), 4626-4639.
[79] Han, Z., & Bi, Y. (2009). Oral spelling and writing in a logographic language: Insights from a Chinese dysgraphic individual. Brain and language, 110(1), 23-28.
[80] Hartmann, K. (2008). Focus and emphasis in tone and intonational languages. The discourse potential of underspecified structures, 389-411.
[81] Henderson, L. M., Baseler, H. A., Clarke, P. J., Watson, S., & Snowling, M. J. (2011). The N400 effect in children: Relationships with comprehension, vocabulary and decoding. Brain and Language, 117(2), 88-99.
[82] Hendrickson, K., Love, T., Walenski, M., & Friend, M. (2019). The organization of words and environmental sounds in the second year: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Developmental science, 22(1), e12746.
[83] Hirst, D., & Cristo, A. D. (Eds.). (1998). Intonation systems: A survey of Twenty Languages. Cambridge University Press.
[84] Hoeks, J. C., Stowe, L. A., & Doedens, G. (2004). Seeing words in context: the interaction of lexical and sentence level information during reading. Cognitive brain research, 19(1), 59-73.
[85] Holcomb, P. J., Coffey, S. A., & Neville, H. J. (1992). Visual and auditory sentence processing: A developmental analysis using event?related brain potentials. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8(2-3), 203-241.
[86] Huang, C. R., & Chen, K. J. (1998). CKIP no. 98-04: Zhongyang yanjiuyuan pingheng yuliaoku [CKIP no. 98-04: Sinica Corpus]. Information Processing Group, Academia Sinica.
[87] Huettig, F., Audring, J., & Jackendoff, R. (2022). A parallel architecture perspective on pre-activation and prediction in language processing. Cognition, 224, 105050.
[88] Hyman, L. M. (2006). Word-prosodic typology. Phonology, 23(2), 225-257.
[89] James, A. L. (1940). Speech signals in telephony. Sir I. Pitman & sons, Limited.
[90] Jones, D. (1976). An Outline of English Phonetics. (9th ed.). Cambridge University Press.
[91] Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and cognitive processes, 15(2), 159-201.
[92] Kamarudin, K. (2020). Speech Acts Analysis and Its Application Within the Interlocutors’Communication.
Cordova Journal: languages and culture studies, 10(1),
67-83.
[93] Kim, A., & Osterhout, L. (2005). The independence of combinatory semantic processing:
Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of memory and language, 52(2), 205-225.
[94] Kim, B., Lee, J., & Lee, G. G. (2010). Fujisaki Model Based Intonation Modeling for Korean TTS System. In Ubiquitous Computing and Multimedia Applications: International Conference, UCMA 2010, Miyazaki, Japan, June 23-25, 2010. Proceedings 1 (pp. 103-111). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
[95] Kim, H., Hwang, H., & Rah, Y. (2017). Young EFL students’reliance on pathbreaking verbs in the use of English argument structure constructions. Journal of Cognitive Science, 18(3), 1-34.
[96] Kimmelman, V. (2015). Topics and topic prominence in two sign languages. Journal of Pragmatics, 87, 156-170.
[97] Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in cognitive sciences, 14(7), 317-324.
[98] Kolk, H. H., Chwilla, D. J., Van Herten, M., & Oor, P. J. (2003). Structure and limited capacity in verbal working memory: A study with event-related potentials. Brain and language, 85(1), 1-36.
[99] Korhonen, A., & Briscoe, T. (2004). Extended lexical-semantic classification of English verbs. In Proceedings of the Computational Lexical Semantics Workshop at HLT-NAACL 2004 (pp. 38-45).
[100] Kroon, S. (2003). Mother tongue and mother tongue education. In World Yearbook of Education 2003 (pp. 47-60). Routledge.
[101] Kuperberg, G. R., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D., & Holcomb, P. J. (2003). Electrophysiological
distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. Cognitive brain research, 17(1), 117-129.
[102] Kuperberg, G. R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax. Brain research, 1146, 23-49.
[103] Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual review of psychology, 62, 621-647.
[104] Leckey, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2020). The P3b and P600 (s): Positive contributions to language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 57(7), e13351.
[105] Lee, L. S. (1998). Structural features of Chinese language –Why Chinese spoken language processing is special and where we are. In Proc. International Symposium on Chinese Language Processing [ISCSLP] (Vol. 98).
[106] Lee, S.-W., & Chen, C.-M. (1993). Zhongwen kouyu yu shuxieyu de bijiao yanjiu [A Comparative Study of Chinese Spoken and Written Language]. Teaching and Research, 15, 63–96.
[107] Levin, B. (1993). English Verb Classes and Alternations: A Preliminary Investigation. University of Chicago Press.
[108] Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1976). Subject and Topic: A New Typology of Language. In Charles N. Li (ed.), Subject and Topic. Academic Press (pp. 457–489).
[109] Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1989). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Univ of California Press.
[110] Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (2022). Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Grammar (S. F. Huang, Trans.; Rev. ed.). Crane Publishing Co., Ltd.
[111] Li, J. (2018). Temporal hierarchy of intonation and tone processing in Mandarin Chinese. PsyArXiv.
[112] Li, Y., Tang, C., Lu, J., Wu, J., & Chang, E. F. (2021). Human cortical encoding of pitch in tonal and non-tonal languages. Nature communications, 12(1), 1-12.
[113] Liao, C. H., & Lau, E. (2020). Enough time to get results? An ERP investigation of prediction with complex events. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(9), 1162-1182.
[114] Lijffijt, M., Lane, S. D., Meier, S. L., Boutros, N. N., Burroughs, S., Steinberg, J. L., ... & Swann, A. C. (2009). P50, N100, and P200 sensory gating: relationships with behavioral inhibition, attention, and working memory. Psychophysiology, 46(5), 1059-1068.
[115] Lin, P.- J. (2002). Shiyong shiyingxing qujian moxing yu yuzhe shuohua suduzhi tiaozheng [Adaptive duration modeling for speaker adaptation of speaking rate] [Unpublished master’s thesis]. Department of science and information engineering, National Cheng Kung University.
[116] Liu, M. J., & Xu, H. L. (1994, August). Zhongwen dongcide chuli: cikuxiaozu dongci xitong ji dongci dacidian zhi bijiao [The Processing of Chinese Verbs: a comparison of the CKIP classification and the Chinese Verb Dictionary]. In Proceedings of Rocling VII Computational Linguistics Conference VII (pp. 91-110).
[117] Luraghi, S., & Parodi, C. (2008). Key terms in syntax and syntactic theory. Bloomsbury Publishing.
[118] Maffongelli, L., Antognini, K., & Daum, M. M. (2018). Syntactical regularities of action sequences in the infant brain: When structure matters. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12682.
[119] Maran, M., Numssen, O., Hartwigsen, G., & Zaccarella, E. (2022). Online neurostimulation of Broca’s area does not interfere with syntactic predictions: A combined TMS-EEG approach to basic linguistic combination. Frontiers in Psychology, 13.
[120] Martin, D., & Miller, C. (2012). Speech and language difficulties in the classroom (2th ed.). David Fulton Publishers.
[121] Martinez, J. L., & De Vera, P. V. (2019). Sociolinguistic Competence of Foreign National College Students. Online Submission, 21, 291-336.
[122] McCornack, S. (2010). Reflect & Relate: An Introduction to Interpersonal Communication(2nd ed.). Bedford/St. Martin’s.
[123] McCornack, S., & Morrison, K. (2018). Reflect & Relate: An Introduction to Interpersonal Communication (5th ed.). Bedford/St. Martin’s.
[124] McQuail, D., & Windahl, S. (2015). Communication Models for the Study of Mass Communications (2nd ed.). Routledge.
[125] McQueen, J. M., & Dilley, L. C. (2020). Prosody and spoken-word recognition. In Gussenhhoven, C., & Chen, A. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language prosody(pp. 509-521). Oxford University Press.
[126] Melynyte, S., Wang, G. Y., & Griskova-Bulanova, I. (2018). Gender effects on auditory P300: A systematic review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 133, 55-65.
[127] Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological review, 63(2), 81.
[128] Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the Structure of Behavior. Henry Holt and Company.
[129] Mixdorff, H., Fujisaki, H., Chen, G. P., & Hu, Y. (2003). Towards the automatic extraction of Fujisaki model parameters for Mandarin. In Eighth European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology.
[130] Molinaro, N., Barber, H. A., Caffarra, S., & Carreiras, M. (2015). On the left anterior negativity (LAN): The case of morphosyntactic agreement. Cortex, 66(156-159), 8.
[131] National Center for Environmental Health (2019, October 7). What Noises Cause Hearing Loss? Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
[132] Nespor, M., Shukla, M., & Mehler, J. (2011). Stress timed vs. syllable timed languages. The Blackwell companion to phonology, 1-13.
[133] Neville, H., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I., & Garrett, M. F. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of cognitive Neuroscience, 3(2), 151-165.
[134] Nguyen, D. T., Luong, C. M., Vu, B. K., Mixdorff, H., & Ngo, H. H. (2004, October). Fujisaki model based F0 contours in Vietnamese TTS. In INTERSPEECH (pp. 1429-1432).
[135] Nickels, S., & Steinhauer, K. (2018). Prosody–syntax integration in a second language: Contrasting event-related potentials from German and Chinese learners of English using linear mixed effect models. Second Language Research, 34(1), 9-37.
[136] Nooteboom, S. (1997). The prosody of speech: melody and rhythm. The handbook of phonetic sciences, 5, 640-673.
[137] Obleser, J., Lahiri, A., & Eulitz, C. (2003). Auditory-evoked magnetic field codes place of articulation in timing and topography around 100 milliseconds post syllable onset. Neuroimage, 20(3), 1839-1847.
[138] Obleser, J., Scott, S. K., & Eulitz, C. (2006). Now you hear it, now you don’t: transient traces of consonants and their nonspeech analogues in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 16(8), 1069-1076.
[139] Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 20(4), 786.
[140] Owens, R. E. (2019). Language development: An Introduction (9th ed.). Pearson Education.
[141] Pam, K. J. (2013). English, a Tonal Language? AFRREV IJAH: An International Journal of Arts and Humanities, 2(1), 273-280.
[142] Parrish, A., & Pylkkanen, L. (2022). Conceptual combination in the LATL with and without syntactic composition. Neurobiology of Language, 3(1), 46-66.
[143] Paul, R. & Norbury, C. F. (2012). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence (4th ed.). Elsevier Health Sciences.
[144] Paul, R., Norbury, C. F. & Gosse, C. (2024). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence (6th ed.). Elsevier Health Sciences.
[145] Payne, T. E. (2007). Summary of Semantic Roles and Grammatical Relations. Eng 595G. press.
[146] Phillips, C., Pellathy, T., Marantz, A., Yellin, E., Wexler, K., Poeppel, D., ... & Roberts, T. (2000). Auditory cortex accesses phonological categories: an MEG mismatch study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1038-1055.
[147] Plank, F. (2017). Split morphology: How agglutination and flexion mix. Linguistic Typology, 21.
[148] Poeppel, D., Phillips, C., Yellin, E., Rowley, H. A., Roberts, T. P., & Marantz, A. (1997). Processing of vowels in supratemporal auditory cortex. Neuroscience letters, 221(2-3), 145-148.
[149] Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical neurophysiology, 118(10), 2128-2148.
[150] Puga, K., Fuchs, R., Setter, J., & Mok, P. (2017). The perception of English intonation patterns by German L2 speakers of English. Proceedings of the Interspeech 2017, 3241-3245.
[151] Pylkkanen, L. (2019). The neural basis of combinatory syntax and semantics. Science, 366(6461), 62-66.
[152] Raymond D. Kent, & Charles Read. (2002). The Acoustic Analysis of Speech (2nd ed.). Singular/Thomson Learning.
[153] Regel, S., Meyer, L., & Gunter, T. C. (2014). Distinguishing neurocognitive processes reflected by P600 effects: Evidence from ERPs and neural oscillations. PloS one, 9(5), e96840.
[154] Rijkhoff, J. (2015). Word order. In International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 644-656). Elsevier.
[155] Roll, M., Horne, M., & Lindgren, M. (2009). Left-edge boundary tone and main clause verb effects on syntactic processing in embedded clauses–An ERP study. Journal of neurolinguistics, 22(1), 55-73.
[156] Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive psychology, 7(4), 573-605.
[157] Rosen, S. T. (2007). Structured Events, Structured Discourse. In Ramchand & Reiss (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces. Oxford University Press.
[158] Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1984). Assessing the pragmatic abilities of children: Part 1. Organizational framework and assessment parameters. Journal of speech and Hearing Disorders, 49(1), 2-11.
[159] Sapungan, R. M., Aceron, R. M., Castillo, R. C., Katigbak, X. N. V., & Villanueva, D. M. (2018). Sociolinguistic Competence of Filipino Hotelier and Restaurateur Interns. International Linguistics Research, 1(2).
[160] Schack, K. (2000). Comparison of intonation patterns in Mandarin and English for a particular speaker. University of Rochester Working Papers in the Language Sciences, 1(1), 24-54.
[161] Schirmer, A., Tang, S. L., Penney, T. B., Gunter, T. C., & Chen, H. C. (2005). Brain responses to segmentally and tonally induced semantic violations in Cantonese. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 17(1), 1-12.
[162] Shakkour, W. (2014). Cognitive skill transfer in English reading acquisition: Alphabetic and logographic languages compared. Open Journal of Modern Linguistics, 4(04), 544.
[163] Sheehan, E. A., Namy, L. L., & Mills, D. L. (2007). Developmental changes in neural activity to familiar words and gestures. Brain and language, 101(3), 246-259.
[164] Shen, W., Fiori-Duharcourt, N., & Isel, F. (2016). Functional significance of the semantic P600: evidence from the event-related brain potential source localization. NeuroReport, 27(7), 548-558.
[165] Shestakova, A., Brattico, E., Soloviev, A., Klucharev, V., & Huotilainen, M. (2004). Orderly cortical representation of vowel categories presented by multiple exemplars. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(3), 342-350.
[166] Shtyrov, Y., Pulvermuller, F., Naatanen, R., & Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2003). Grammar processing outside the focus of attention: an MEG study. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 15(8), 1195-1206.
[167] Slavova, V., & Soschen, A. (2015). On mental representations: Language structure and meaning revised. International Journal Information theories & applications, 2(4), 316-325.
[168] Song, J. J. (2012). Word Order: Research Surveys in Linguistics. Cambridge University Press.
[169] Steinhauer, K., & Drury, J. E. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and language, 120(2), 135-162.
[170] Stuckenschneider, T., Askew, C. D., Weber, J., Abeln, V., Rudiger, S., Summers, M. J., ... & NeuroExercise Study Group. (2020). Auditory event-related potentials in individuals with subjective and mild cognitive impairment. Behavioural brain research, 391, 112700.
[171] Trask, R. L. (1995). A Dictionary of Phonetics and Phonology (1st ed.). Routledge.
[172] Tse, K. P. (2022). Yuyanxue gailun [An Introduction to Linguistics] (4th ed.). San-Min Book Co., Ltd.
[173] Tseng, C. Y. & Su, Z. Y. (2008, September). Mandarin Discourse Prosody Other than Tones and Intonation–Decomposing the F0 Constitution by Prosodic Hierarchy with the Fujisaki Model [In Chinese]. In Proceedings of the 20th Conference on Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing (pp. 53-65).
[174] Tye Murray, N. (2014). Foundations of aural rehabilitation: Children, adults, and their family members (4th ed.). Cengage.
[175] Ueno, M., & Garnsey, S. M. (2008). An ERP study of the processing of subject and object relative clauses in Japanese. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(5), 646-688.
[176] University of Minnesota (Ed.). (2016). Communication in the Real World: An Introduction to Communication Studies. University of Minnesota Libraries Publishing.
[177] Van Dinteren, R., Arns, M., Jongsma, M. L., & Kessels, R. P. (2014). P300 development across the lifespan: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS one, 9(2), e87347.
[178] Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J. (2012). Prediction during language comprehension:
Benefits, costs, and ERP components. International journal of psychophysiology, 83(2), 176-190.
[179] Van Trijp, R. (2011). A design pattern for argument structure constructions. Design patterns in Fluid Construction Grammar, 115-145.
[180] Verleger, R. (2008). P3b: towards some decision about memory. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(4), 968-970.
[181] Wambacq, I. J., & Jerger, J. F. (2004). Processing of affective prosody and lexicalsemantics in spoken utterances as differentiated by event-related potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(3), 427-437.
[182] Wang, Y., Takahiro, H., Jeong, D. Y. (2016). Jiaodian he jumoyingao de hengding bianyi ji qixiangguan wenti [Stability and Variation of Sentence Focus and Ending Pitches and Relevant Issues]. Essays on Linguistics, 54(2), 66-99.
[183] Whaley, L. J. (1996). Introduction to typology: The unity and diversity of language. SAGE publications.
[184] Wu, J. (2007) Jiaose zhicheng yufa jianjie [An Introduction to Role and Reference Grammar]. Language and Linguistics, 8(1), 5-70.
[185] Yan, Y. (2023). Grammaticalization of Coverbs in Modern Mandarin Chinese. Lecture Notes on Language and Literature, 6(15).
[186] Yang, H. K., Kim, R., & Sung, M. C. (2014). Basic communicative competence and sentential utterance production. The SNU Journal of Education Research, 23,
98-117.
[187] Yang, S., Cai, Y., Xie, W., & Jiang, M. (2021). Semantic and syntactic processing during comprehension: ERP evidence from Chinese QING structure. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 15, 701923.
[188] Yang, Y. C. H., & Kuo, J. J. (1998, February). The Chinese temporal coverbs, postpositions, coverb-postposition pairs, and their temporal logic. In Proceedings of the 12th Pacific Asia Conference on Language, Information and Computation (pp.20-32).
[189] Yang, Y. F., Huang, X. J., Gao, L. (2006). Studies on Speech Prosody [In Chinese]. Advances in Psychological Science, 14(4), 546-550.
[190] Ye, Z., Luo, Y. J., Friederici, A. D., & Zhou, X. (2006). Semantic and syntactic processing in Chinese sentence comprehension: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain research, 1071(1), 186-196.
[191] Ye, Z., & Zhou, X. (2009). Conflict control during sentence comprehension: fMRI evidence. Neuroimage, 48(1), 280-290.
[192] Yin, H. (2003). A cognitive account of Mandarin coverbs. Working Papers of the Linguistics Circle, 17, 201-208.
[193] You, J. M., & Chen, K. J. (2004, July). Automatic semantic role assignment for a tree structure. In Proceedings of the third SIGHAN workshop on Chinese language processing (pp. 109-115).
[194] Yuan, Y. L. (2002). Lunyuan jiaose de cengji guanxi han yuyi tezheng [On the hierarchical relation and semantic features of the thematic roles in Chinese]. Chinese Teaching in the World, 2(3), 11-22.
[195] Zhang, J. (2023). Chinese syllable structure. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics.
[196] Zora, H., Wester, J., & Csepe, V. (2023). Predictions about prosody facilitate lexical
access: Evidence from P50/N100 and MMN components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 194, 112262. |